Skip to main content

Proximal and distal predictors of self-regulatory change in children aged 4 to 7 years

Abstract

Background

Growth in early self-regulation skills has been linked to positive health, wellbeing, and achievement trajectories across the lifespan. While individual studies have documented specific influences on self-regulation competencies in early childhood, few have modelled a comprehensive range of predictors of self-regulation change across health, development, and environment simultaneously. This study aimed to examine the concurrent associations among a range of proximal and distal influences on change in children’s self-regulation skills over 2 years from age 4–5 years.

Methods

Data from the Longitudinal Study of Australian Children (N = 4983) were used in a structural equation model, predicting a multi-source composite measure of self-regulation at each of 4–5 years and 6–7 years. By controlling for earlier self-regulation and covariates, the model examined the relative contributions of a comprehensive range of variables to self-regulation change including health, development, educational, home environment, time-use, and neighbourhood characteristics.

Results

The significant predictors of children’s self-regulation growth across 4 to 7 years were fewer behavioural sleep problems, higher gross motor and pre-academic skills, lower levels of maternal and paternal angry parenting, and lower levels of financial hardship. There were also marginal effects for high-quality home learning environments and child-educator relationships.

Conclusion

Findings suggest that if we are to successfully foster children’s self-regulation skills, interventionists would do well to operate not only on children’s current capacities but also key aspects of their surrounding context.

Peer Review reports

Background

Self-regulation refers to the ability to exert control over our cognition, emotion, and behaviour in ways that are adaptive to functioning. These skills develop across the lifespan, but most rapidly in early childhood alongside cortical maturation processes. In terms of self-regulation development, early improvements appear to be better, with strong early childhood self-regulation skills linked with a wide range of health and achievement outcomes across the lifespan, including positive mental and physical health, and educational attainment [1,2,3]. In contrast, poorer self-regulation in early childhood has been linked with school adjustment difficulties [4], behaviour problems [5], adolescent risk-taking [2], and adult disordered behaviour [6].

Early childhood is a period in which growth in self-regulation is not only particularly desirable, but also demonstrably possible. In fact, growth in self-regulation skills in the early years of life (controlling for early self-regulatory levels and family environment) has been found to reduce risk of childhood behaviour problems [5], adolescent crime, self-harm, and mental health problems [2], as well as enhance academic learning trajectories [7]. Given limited understanding of antecedents of early self-regulation change that can shift trajectories and outcomes more broadly, intervention approaches remain incongruous. For instance, approaches to self-regulation intervention include computerized executive function training, specialized preschool curricula, physical activities, arts and music, motor skill development, and so forth [8,9,10]. While it is indeed likely that multiple approaches will be effective, and ideally suited to different contexts, needs and children, the design of interventions would nevertheless be improved through a more comprehensive and holistic understanding of early childhood factors and experiences that support self-regulation development.

The individual and environmental conditions that support optimal development in self-regulation across early childhood remain relatively unclear. Various lines of inquiry have identified longitudinal predictors associated with better point-in-time self-regulation in early childhood including rich home learning environments [11], positive parenting approaches [12], stronger motor [13] and language development [14], and well-adjusted sleep behaviours [15]. However, very few studies have examined the extent to which these, and other plausible proximal and distal factors predict change in self-regulation over time. The aim of this study is to investigate the concurrent associations among a range of proximal and distal influences on change in children’s self-regulation skills over 2 years beginning at 4–5 years of age.

Methods

Participants

This study used data from the population-representative Kindergarten (K) cohort of the Longitudinal Study of Australian Children (LSAC), with full study design details described elsewhere [16]. In brief, for the K cohort, 4983 children aged 4–5-years old were recruited in 2004 with biennial data collection occurring since then. Data collection involves parent and teacher questionnaires, computer assisted interviews with parents and children, and direct assessments with children. The current study uses data collected for the K cohort across two waves (when children were 4- to 5-years old and 6- to 7-years old). Table 1 describes the characteristics of the sample.

Table 1 Sample characteristics

Measures

Self-regulation was assessed at 4–5 and 6–7 years of age using a factor score we have previously established as a reliable indicator of children’s self-regulatory capacity with good predictive validity of broad later-life outcomes into adolescence [2]. A total of 20 survey items from parent-, teacher-, and observer-report ratings of self-regulation were standardized and then averaged to create a single composite score (M = 0, SD = 1). Constituent items of this factor index the extent to which children can control and sustain their attention, and control their behaviour and emotions (see Table 2). Internal consistency was high (alpha = 0.84 at 4–5 years, 0.86 at 6–7 years).

Table 2 Items included in the self-regulation measure at 4–5 years and 6–7 years

Predictors of self-regulation change were selected from the domains of health, development, home environment, education, time use, and neighbourhood measured when children were 4–5-years old. Details of each of these are provided in Table 3. Where parent-report is indicated, this was provided by Parent 1 (defined by LSAC as the parent who knows the study child best, which in 97% of cases was the mother).

Table 3 Predictors of self-regulation growth in the model

Control variables included in the analyses were gender, age of assessment (in months) at baseline, birth weight percentile, whether or not the child had ever been breastfed, Aboriginal and Torres Strait Islander status, Non-English speaking home background, maternal education level (on a 6-point scale from incomplete high school to postgraduate degree), and household income bracket. We used data from the age 4–5 years data collection for these variables, providing the most complete data possible (before attrition in the longitudinal study).

Approach to analysis and missing data

A structural equation model was tested in Mplus version 7.11. Figure 1 depicts the model, showing self-regulation at 6–7 years predicted by the full range of variables described above, while controlling for self-regulation measured two years earlier. This approach to modelling means the estimates for the predictors represent their impact on residualized change in self-regulation from 4 to 7 years of age, because the effect of the earlier measure of self-regulation has already been accounted for. Additionally, effects of stable covariates present from birth on earlier self-regulation were controlled for. Correlations among all predictor variables were included in the model, with the strongest significant correlation as r = .45 for the correlation among teacher-reported gross motor and fine motor skills. Due to the large sample size, we use a conservative p value of < .01 to indicate a significant effect and < .02 for a marginally significant effect.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Conceptual model tested through structural equation model analyses

The amount of missing data varied across waves and variables, ranging from no missing data for socio-demographic characteristics at 4–5 years to 45% missing data for the self-regulation scores at 4–5 and 6–7 years due primarily to item-level missing data from teacher non-report. The data were considered missing at random (MAR) because it was unlikely that the presence of a missing value was related to the response that would have been given [31]. We used full information maximum likelihood with a robust estimator to address missing data, allowing us to retain 98% of the sample in the statistical models. We used the sampling weights provided for LSAC [32] to account for sampling error.

Results

The model was a good fit for the data and accounted for 42% of variance in self-regulation at 6–7 years, with all estimates shown in Table 4. Self-regulation skills at 6–7 years, after controlling for self-regulation skills at 4–5 years, were predicted by fewer behavioural sleep problems, higher gross motor and pre-academic skills, lower levels of maternal and paternal angry parenting, and lower levels of financial hardship. There were also marginal effects for the home learning environment and child-educator relationships. Covariates associated with stronger self-regulatory skills at 6–7 years including being a female, having a higher birthweight percentile, identifying as non-Aboriginal and Torres Strait Islander, having a mother with a higher level of education, and a higher household income.

Table 4 Standardized coefficients for the predictors of self-regulation at 6–7 years controlling for prior self-regulation and covariates

Discussion

This is the first paper to model a comprehensive and concurrent set of predictors across health, development, and environment in relation to self-regulatory development of young children, across a two-year period beginning from age 4–5 years. Controlling for a range of background factors, significant predictors of self-regulatory growth included: fewer behavioural sleep problems; higher gross motor and pre-academic skills; lower levels of maternal and paternal angry parenting; lower levels of financial hardship; and marginal effects for home learning environment and child-educator relationships. As predictors were modelled simultaneously, significant findings provide a (likely conservative) estimate of the associations between each variable and self-regulation change, over and above the combined associations of all other variables in the model. While previous studies have provided insight into the transactional mechanisms between some factors known to influence self-regulation (e.g. parenting and sleep), this model better reflects the complexity of children’s lives and the combined impact of a range of factors on self-regulatory change. Thus the study makes an important contribution toward prevention and intervention efforts by identifying the most salient and high-potential factors to target for self-regulation interventionists taking a holistic approach to supporting self-regulatory growth in young children.

Substantial research and theory supports both acute and persistent associations of self-regulation with learning and academic skills [33] with self-regulation typically positioned as a predictor of academic skills. In a related finding, but with self-regulation as the outcome, in our model pre-academic skills were one of the strongest predictors of self-regulation growth. It is clear why self-regulation would predict learning and academic skills: the ability to direct and sustain attention, tackle new challenges, resist maladaptive impulses, and work collaboratively and pro-socially with others – all hallmarks of high self-regulation – serve to support on-task behaviour, effort and persistence during learning. However, there is comparatively less research focused on the possible reciprocal effects with pre-academic skills predicting self-regulation growth. A number of explanations are feasible. First, it is likely that self-regulation and early literacy and numeracy skills, as represented by our pre-academic skill assessment, develop in a bidirectional manner across early childhood [34, 35]. For example, time spent in focussed literacy and numeracy learning activities provides the opportunity to extend and enhance self-regulatory capacities, particularly in attentional and cognitive control aspects. It is likely that had we had an earlier and multiple measures of both self-regulation and early concept comprehension, literacy, and numeracy, we would have established birdirectional and reciprocal associations across time. A second and related explanation is that the pre-academic assessment used here may have tapped children’s visual-motor skills given it was a pencil and paper task requiring the writing of letters. While there was no visual-motor data available for children in this dataset, scores on the pre-academic test did correlate (r = .40) with the fine motor variable in our model (single item of teacher report of fine motor competence). Recent research has suggested that visual-motor skills and cognitive self-regulation, as enabled by executive functions, co-develop in a bidirectional manner [35] and it may be that our findings are reflecting a small portion of this transactional process at this period of development. That is, children who scored more highly on the pre-academic score may have done so due to higher visual-motor skills, which may themselves co-develop with and support self-regulatory growth.

Pre-school gross motor abilities were also significantly, albeit modestly, associated with children’s self-regulation growth. This is consistent with suggestions of common mechanisms (i.e., executive functions) that are implicated in both self-regulation and motor learning [36,37,38], such that both show common areas of neural activation, are impaired after damage to neural regions for the other, and are often both impaired in cognitive disorders, such as ADHD and dyslexia. Indeed, tasks that are motor-demanding for young children, such as navigating uneven surfaces and/or obstacles, are more cognitively demanding and lead to more cognitive errors than less cognitively demanding motor tasks [39]. As such, one possibility is that this finding is indicative of the concomitance between self-regulatory and motor skills. However, that gross motor skills were associated with change in self-regulation may additionally suggest that the acquisition of motor proficiency creates new learning opportunities [40] such as experiences that serve to foster self-regulation (e.g., increased mobility causing children to encounter rules associated with access, involvement in physically active shared play providing opportunities for impulse control and turn-taking, etc.) As such, gross motor skills may open a gateway to important self-regulation-promoting experiences and activities, whereas low levels of gross motor skills might consume much of the cognitive resource that otherwise could be directed toward these same activities.

Another factor that was modestly but significantly and uniquely related to self-regulation growth was sleep problems. This aligns with a large body of existing research that identifies sleep problems as a key contributor to daytime self-regulatory problems in young children both in the short [41] and long term [18, 42]. It is possible that behavioural sleep problems in young children reflect an underlying phenotype associated with regulatory problems [43, 44], and/or that early behavioural sleep problems initiate a developmental cascade that disrupts emotional and attentional development over time [15]. Either way, brief sleep interventions are known to be safe and effective in improving both sleep behaviours and daytime self-regulatory functioning in young children in both typically-developing [45,46,47] and clinical populations [48, 49].

Our finding that angry parenting was associated with less growth in self-regulation for children echoes a range of prior studies that have linked aggressive, controlling parenting with poor self-regulation in children [50,51,52,53,54]. However, this study extends that work by including not only mothers’ but also fathers’ parenting, a rare inclusion. We suggest that angry parenting as measured here is indicative of dysregulated parenting, and potentially of overall emotional regulation skills of parents. Mechanisms through which this might limit self-regulatory growth in children include heritability pathways in terms of self-regulation capabilities [55], and socialisation pathways in which children learn about self-regulatory behaviours through modelling their parents’ behaviours. It is also important to note that child-driven effects are possible, as reflected in prior studies that show dysregulation in young children is associated with increased parenting stress and more-negative parenting approaches [56, 57]. These bidirectional relationships between parenting and children’s self-regulation, which are likely to establish mutual promotion/exacerbation processes over time, were not modelled in this study and should be the focus of future longitudinal work.

A number of socioeconomic variables were associated with enhanced self-regulatory growth including higher household incomes, higher maternal education levels and living in households with lower levels of financial hardship. The experience of significant financial hardships such as those tapped here is likely associated with stressful home environments, which impact on children’s physiology and neurodevelopment in ways that limit their capacity for self-regulation development [58, 59]. Indeed, early self-regulation has been identified as one of the foremost mechanisms through which early stressors and socioeconomic disadvantage can lead to poorer academic and wellbeing outcomes [60]. For these reasons, much of the prevention and intervention focus to date has been on children from disadvantaged backgrounds in an effort to address socio-economic gradients in achievement likely mediated through early self-regulatory capacity. Our findings suggest this focus is well-placed.

Marginal effects were also found for the association between educator-child relationships and the home learning environment, with self-regulatory change. The finding regarding importance of educator-child relationship in terms of children’s early self-regulation development reflects other similar findings in both Australia [61] and Europe [62]. Positive student-teacher relationships likely matter because they set the context within which teachers can enact strategies particularly important for acquiring self-regulation during the preschool developmental period [63] including co-regulation, modelling and coaching [64]. Our findings regarding the home learning environment align with a prior American longitudinal study linking parental involvement in home learning activities with children’s self-regulatory development [65].

Limitations

Although this study included a comprehensive array of predictors of self-regulation growth across a specific period in early childhood, there are a number of limitations related primarily to measurement. Most measures were broad and blunt instruments of their constructs. This reflects the nature of the population dataset, in which a broad spectrum of measures capturing child development and the environment were desired, rather than an in-depth measurement of any particular constructs. In addition, our self-regulation composite was only available at two time points in this dataset, meaning that more sophisticated growth curve modelling, which requires a minimum of three time points, could not be undertaken. It is also important to note that although we included a wide array of predictors, nearly 60% of the variance in our self-regulation composite at 6–7 years was still unexplained by the model. This suggests that even large-scale studies such as these are missing key ingredients related to self-regulatory growth. Our understandings could be enhanced through studies which capture potential variables that are not often measured, including chronic stress (e.g. cortisol), psychophysiological arousal and regulation, sensory processing, and more detailed understandings of the nature of home learning and early education and care activities. Finally, it is important to note that participants in this study were recruited in 2004. While it is anticipated that there has been limited change in most lifestyle factors investigated (e.g., parenting), new cohort studies are required to better understand the influence of more prominent societal change such as increased access and use of digital devices.

Conclusion

While we know that self-regulation is important for a broad range of longitudinal achievement and wellbeing outcomes, and that early childhood is a key window for self-regulatory growth, we have not yet been overly effective in intervention efforts. One reason for this might be that we need more holistic and evidence-informed theories and approaches to self-regulatory development, rather than a focus on single factors that appear predictive in isolation. We need more complex modelling of the interactions between these various factors and their association with self-regulation change (not just prediction at one time point). The findings of this study suggest a starting point for further detailed research that aims to achieve this.

Availability of data and materials

The dataset analysed for the current study is available from the Australian Data Archive https://dataverse.ada.edu.au/dataset.xhtml?persistentId=doi:10.26193/JOZW2U

Abbreviations

K cohort:

Kindergarten cohort

LSAC:

Longitudinal Study of Australian Children

References

  1. McClelland MM, Acock AC, Piccinin A, Rhea SA, Stallings MC. Relations between preschool attention span-persistence and age 25 educational outcomes. Early Child Res Q. 2013;28:314–24.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Howard SJ, Williams KE. Early self-regulation, early self-regulatory change, and their longitudinal relation sto adolescents' academic, health, and mental well-being outcomes. J Dev Behav Pediatrics. 2018;39:489–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Moffitt TE, Arseneault L, Belsky J, Dickson N, Hancox R, Harington H, et al. A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2011;108(7):2693–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Williams KE, Nicholson JM, Walker S, Berthelsen D. Early childhood profiles of sleep problems and self-regulation predict later school adjustment. Br J Educ Psychol. 2016;86(2):331–50.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Sawyer ACP, Miller-Lewis LR, Searle AK, Sawyer MG, Lynch JW. Is greater improvement in early self-regulation associated with fewer behavioral problems later in childhood? Dev Psychol. 2015;51(12):1740–55.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Slutske WS, Moffitt TE, Poulton R, Caspi A. Undercontrolled temperament at age 3 predicts disordered gambling at age 32: a longitudinal study of a complete birth cohort. Psychol Sci. 2012;23(5):510–6.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Sawyer ACP, Chittleborough CR, Mittinty MN, Miller-Lewis LR, Sawyer MG, Sullivan T, et al. Are trajectories of self-regulation abilities from ages 2-3 to 6-7 associated with academic achievement in the early school years? Child: Care Health Dev. 2014;41:744–54.

    Google Scholar 

  8. Takacs ZK, Kassai R. The efficacy of different interventions to foster children’s executive function skills: a series of meta-analyses. Psychol Bull. 2019;145(7):653–97.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Diamond A. Activities and programs that improve children’s executive functions. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2012;21(5):335–41.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Jacob R, Parkinson J. The potential for school-based interventions that target executive function to improve academic achievement: a review. Rev Educ Res. 2015;85(4):512–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Baker CE, Cameron CE, Rimm-Kaufman SE, Grissmer D. Family and sociodemographic predictors of school readiness among African American boys in kindergarten. Early Educ Dev. 2012;23(6):833–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Hindman AH, Morrison FJ. Differential contributions of three parenting dimensions to preschool literacy and social skills in a middle-income sample. Merrill-Palmer Q. 2012;58(2):191–223.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Kim H, Byers AI, Cameron CE, Brock LL, Cottone EA, Grissmer DW. Unique contributions of attentional control and visuomotor integration on concurrent teacher-reported classroom functioning in early elementary students. Early Child Res Q. 2016;36:379–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Hanno E, Surrain S. The direct and indirect relations between self-regulation and language development among monolinguals and dual language learners. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev. 2019;22:75–89.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Williams KE, Berthelsen D, Walker S, Nicholson JM. A developmental cascade model of behavioral sleep problems and emotional and attentional self-regulation across early childhood. Behav Sleep Med. 2017;15(1):1–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Soloff C, Lawrence D, Johnstone R. The Longitudinal Study of Australian Children LSAC Technical Paper 1: Sample design.: Australian Government Department of Family and Community Services; 2005.

  17. Varni JW, Seid M, Rode CA. The PedsQL: measurement model for the pediatric quality of life inventory. Med Care. 1999;37(2):126–39.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Quach J, Nguyen CD, Williams KE, Sciberras E. Bidirectional associations between child sleep problems and internalizing and externalizing difficulties from preschool to early adolescence. JAMA Pediatr. 2018;172(2):e174363.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Dunn LM, Dunn LM. Examiner’s manual for the Peabody picture vocabulary test (3rd edn.). Circle Pines: American Guidance Service; 1997.

    Google Scholar 

  20. de Lemos M, Doig B. Who am I developmental assessment manual. Melbourne: ACER Press; 2000.

    Google Scholar 

  21. Australian Council for Educational Research. Who Am I Supplementary Information; 2007.

    Google Scholar 

  22. Zubrick SR, Lucas RE, Westrupp EM, Nicholson JM. Growing up in Australia: The Longitudinal Study of Australian Children (LSAC). LSAC Technical Paper No. 12. Parenting measures in the Longitudinal Study of Australian Children: construct validity and measurement quality, Waves 1 to 4. Canberra: Australian Government Department of Social Services; 2014.

  23. Statistics Canada. National Longitudinal Survey of Children & Youth Cycle 3 Survey Instruments 1998–99. Book 1 - Parent & Child; 1999.

    Google Scholar 

  24. Furukawa TA, Kessler RC, Slade T, Andrews G. The performance of the K6 and K10 screening scales for psychological distress in the Australian National Survey of mental health and well-being. Psychol Med. 2003;33(2):357–62.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Hayes N, Berthelsen DC, Nicholson JM, Walker S. Trajectories of parental involvement in home learning activities across the early years: associations with socio-demographic characteristics and children’s learning outcomes. Early Child Dev Care. 2018;188(10):1405–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Spencer N, Strazdins L. Socioeconomic disadvantage and onset of childhood chronic disabling conditions: a cohort study. Arch Dis Child. 2015;100:317–22.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Lewis AJ, Olsson CA. Early life stress and child temperament style as predictors of childhood anxiety and depressive symptoms: Findings from the Longitudinal Study of Australian Children. Depress Res Treat. 2011:296026.

  28. Pianta R. Student–teacher relationship scale. Psychological Assessment Resources: Odessa; 2001.

    Google Scholar 

  29. Gialamas A, Sawyer ACP, Mittinty MN, Zubrick SR, Sawyer MG, Lynch J. Quality of childcare influences children's attentiveness and emotional regulation at school entry. J Pediatr. 2014;165:813–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Australian Bureau of Statistics. Socio-Economic Indexes for Areas Canberra, Australia: Australian Bureau of Statistics; 2018 [updated 27 March 2018. Available from: https://www.abs.gov.au/websitedbs/censushome.nsf/home/seifa.

  31. Enders C. Applied missing data analysis. New York: Guildford Press; 2010.

    Google Scholar 

  32. Soloff C, Lawrence D, Misson S, Johnstone R. The Longitudinal Study of Australian Children LSAC Technical paper No.3: Wave 1 weighting and non-response; 2006.

    Google Scholar 

  33. Robson DA, Allen MS, Howard SJ. Self-regulation in childhood as a predictor of future outcomes: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin. 2020; Epub Jan 6.

  34. Fuhs MW, Nesbitt KT, Farran DC, Dong N. Longitudinal associations between executive functioning and academic skills across content areas. Dev Psychol. 2014;50(6):1698–709.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. McClelland MM, Cameron CE. Developing together: the role of executive function and motor skills in children's academic lives. Early Childhood Res Quarterl. 2019;46:143–51.

    Google Scholar 

  36. Cook CJ, Howard SJ, Scerif G, Twine R, Kahn K, Norris SA, et al. Associations of physical activity and gross motor skills with executive function in preschool children from low-income south african settings. Dev Sci. 2019;22:e12820.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Diamond A. Close interrelation of motor development and cognitive development and of the cerebellum and prefrontal cortex. Child Dev. 2000;71(1):44–56.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Oberer N, Gashaj V, Roebers CM. Motor skills in kindergarten: internal structure, cognitive correlates and relationships to background variables. Hum Mov Sci. 2017;52:170–80.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Berger SE. Locomotor expertise predicts infants’ perseverative errors. Dev Psychol. 2010;46(2):326–36.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Campos JJ, Kermoian R, Zumbahlen MR. Socioemotional transformations in the family system following infant crawling onset. In: Eisenberg N, Fabes RA, editors. Emotion and its regulation in early development. New directions for child development, No. 55: The Jossey-Bass education series; ISSN: 0195–2269 (Print). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass; 1992. p. 25–40.

    Google Scholar 

  41. Wilson KE, Lumeng JC, Kaciroti N, Chen SY-P, LeBourgeois MK, Chervin RD, et al. Sleep hygiene practices and bedtime resistance in low-income preschoolers: does temperament matter? Behav Sleep Med. 2015;13(5):412–23.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Van den Bergh BRH, Mulder EJH. Fetal sleep organization: a biological precursor of self-regulation in childhood and adolescence? Biol Psychol. 2012;89(3):584–90.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Melegari MG, Sette S, Vittori E, Mallia L, Devoto A, Lucidi F, et al. Relations Between Sleep and Temperament in Preschool Children With ADHD. Journal Of Attention Disorders. 2018;24(4):535–44.

  44. Williams KE, Sciberras E. Sleep and self-regulation from birth to 7 years: a retrospective study of children with and without attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder at 8 to 9 years. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2016;37(5):385–94.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Price AMH, Wake M, Ukoumunne OC, Hiscock H. Five-year follow-up of harms and benefits of behavioral infant sleep intervention: randomized trial. Pediatrics. 2012;130(4):643–51.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Price AMH, Wake M, Ukoumunne OC, Hiscock H. Outcomes at six years of age for children with infant sleep problems: longitudinal community-based study. Sleep Med. 2012;13(8):991–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Quach J, Hiscock H, Ukoumunne OC, Wake M. A brief sleep intervention improves outcomes in the school entry year: a randomized controlled trial. Pediatrics. 2011;128(4):692–701.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Papadopoulos N, Sciberras E, Hiscock H, Mulraney M, McGillivray J, Rinehart N. The efficacy of a brief behavioral sleep intervention in school-aged children with ADHD and comorbid autism spectrum disorder. J Atten Disord. 2019;23(4):341–50.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Hiscock H, Sciberras E, Mensah FK, Gerner B, Efron D, Khano S, et al. Impact of a behavioural sleep intervention on symptoms and sleep in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, and parental mental health: randomised controlled trial. BMJ. 2015;350(h68):1–14. https://www.bmj.com/content/350/bmj.h68.

  50. Graziano PA, Calkins SD, Keane SP. Sustained attention development during the toddlerhood to preschool period: associations with toddlers' emotion regulation strategies and maternal behaviour. Infant Child Dev. 2011;20(6):389–408.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  51. Nelson JA, O'Brien M, Calkins SD, Leerkes EM, Marcovitch S, Blankson AN. Maternal expressive style and children's emotional development. Infant Child Dev. 2012;21(3):267–86.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Olson SL, Lopez-Duran N, Lunkenheimer ES, Chang H, Sameroff AJ. Individual differences in the development of early peer aggression: integrating contributions of self-regulation, theory of mind, and parenting. Dev Psychopathol. 2011;23(1):253–66.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  53. Spinrad TL, Eisenberg N, Silva KM, Eggum ND, Reiser M, Edwards A, et al. Longitudinal relations among maternal behaviors, effortful control and young children's committed compliance. Dev Psychol. 2012;48(2):552–66.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Williams KE, Berthelsen D. The development of prosocial behaviour in early childhood: contributions of early parenting and self-regulation. Int J Early Childhood. 2017;49(1):73–94.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. Scott BG, Lemery-Chalfant K, Clifford S, Tein J-Y, Stoll R, Goldsmith HH. A twin factor mixture modeling approach to childhood temperament: differential heritability. Child Dev. 2016;87(6):1940–55.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  56. Barnes JC, Boutwell BB, Beaver KM, Gibson CL. Analyzing the Origins of Childhood Externalizing Behavioral Problems. Dev Psychol. 2013;49(12):2272–84.

  57. Healey DM, Flory JD, Miller CJ, Halperin JM. Maternal positive parenting style is associated with better functioning in hyperactive/inattentive preschool children. Infant Child Dev. 2011;20(2):148–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Blair C, Raver C, Granger D, Mills-Koonce R, Hibel LC, Investigators FLP. Allostasis and allostatic load in the context of poverty in early childhood. Dev Psychopathol. 2011;23(3):845–57.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  59. Farah M. The neuroscience of socioeconomic status: correlates, causes, and consequences. Neuron. 2017;96(1):56–71.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Blair C, Raver CC. School readiness and self-regulation: a developmental psychobiological approach. Annu Rev Psychol. 2015;66:711–31.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Gialamas A, Alyssa CP, Sawyer A, Mittinty MN, Zubrick SR, Sawyer MG, et al. Quality of childcare influences children's attentiveness and emotional regulation at school entry. J Pedatrics. 2014;165(4):813–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Cadima J, Verschueren K, Leal T, Guedes C. Classroom interactions, dyadic teacher–child relationships, and self–regulation in socially disadvantaged young children. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2016;44(1):7–17.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Kopp CB. Antecedents of self-regulation: a developmental perspective. Dev Psychol. 1982;18(2):199–214.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  64. Silkenbeumer JR, Schiller E-M, Kärtner J. Co- and self-regulation of emotions in the preschool setting. Early Child Res Q. 2018;44:72–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  65. Baker CE. Fathers' and mothers' home literacy involvement and children's cognitive and social emotional development: implications for family literacy programs. Appl Dev Sci. 2013;17(4):184–97.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Funding

There was no funding attached to this study.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

KW co-conceptualised the study, undertook all final analyses and was a major contributor to the writing of the paper. SH created the key outcome composite variables for self-regulation, co-conceptualised the study and was a major contributor to the writing of the paper. All authors have read and approved the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Kate E. Williams.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Ethics approval for participants in the Longitudinal Study of Australia Children was approved by the Human Research Ethics Committee of the Australian Institute of Family Studies.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Williams, K.E., Howard, S.J. Proximal and distal predictors of self-regulatory change in children aged 4 to 7 years. BMC Pediatr 20, 226 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12887-020-02133-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12887-020-02133-6

Keywords