Skip to main content

Clinical profile of reading ability and reading and writing achievement of children with borderline full-scale intellectual quotient: a prospective study

Abstract

Background

Poor reading ability is one of the common causes of low academic performance. In previous studies, children with dyslexia were found to demonstrate poor academic achievement due to poor reading ability. However, the relationship between academic achievement and reading ability in children with a borderline full-scale intellectual quotient (FSIQ) is unknown. This study aimed to clarify the clinical characteristics of children with borderline FSIQ and poor reading ability, and differentiate these characteristics from those of children with higher FSIQ and poor reading ability.

Methods

A total of 126 children (aged 6–15 years) identified as having low academic performance were enrolled. The reading ability of children was assessed through their performance on the hiragana (Japanese syllabary) reading task, while their reading and writing achievement was assessed through their reading and writing score on the Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children, Second Edition. Children were categorized into two groups based on their FSIQ score (FSIQ > 85 and 85 ≥ FSIQ ≥ 70). Reading ability in children was evaluated by referring to the linear relationship between FSIQ and the standard deviation value of reading tasks in typically developing children. A one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) was performed to examine clinical characteristics between higher and lower FSIQ groups. Associations between reading and writing achievement, reading ability, and ages of children were assessed using Pearson’s product-moment correlation coefficients for the higher and lower FSIQ groups.

Results

Poorer reading and writing achievement was associated with poorer reading ability in the higher FSIQ group. Conversely, poorer reading and writing achievement and poor reading ability were associated with older age in the lower FSIQ group.

Conclusions

Poor reading and writing achievement were associated with older age, not with poor reading ability in the lower FSIQ group. Children with lower FSIQ need appropriate educational interventions based on independent assessments to further their academic achievement and reading ability. Moreover, they need more frequent evaluations of their academic achievement than do children with higher FSIQ and poor reading ability since they are more likely to be at a lower academic achievement level at an older age.

Peer Review reports

Background

There are many causes for low academic performance in school-aged children [1,2,3,4,5,6]. For those with developmental dyslexia, a neurodevelopmental disorder, poor reading ability is one of the most common and severe causes for it [4,5,6].

Developmental dyslexia is the most common specific learning disorder, characterized by difficulties in learning to decode (read aloud) and spell. The prevalence of dyslexia among children at the end of their primary education has been determined to be approximately 4–5 % in both Dutch and American population-based studies [7]. A national longitudinal survey in Japan revealed that less than 2 % of children had reading delay/impairment by the time they reached the final grade of primary school. The prevalence of dyslexia in Japan was estimated to be lower than that in foreign countries in which alphabets were used [8]. According to the International Dyslexia Association, dyslexia is marked by impairment in accurate and/or fluent word recognition, and spelling and decoding abilities. Even though children with dyslexia have the same educational and sociocultural resources as typically developing children (TDC), their reading abilities are below the levels expected for their chronological ages [4]. In Japan, children with dyslexia are diagnosed by poor reading ability (prolonged reading time) of hiragana (Japanese syllabary), frequent overall poor reading and writing performance with mixed Chinese character and hiragana than TDC; therefore, acquirement of vocabulary and knowledge is restricted [9]. It has been speculated that one of the main causes of reading disabilities is impairment in phonological processing [10]. Children with dyslexia have been found to demonstrate poor academic achievement due to poor reading ability [4,5,6].

In addition, similar reading disabilities compared to those of children with dyslexia have been found in children with a low full-scale intellectual quotient (FSIQ) in previous studies [10,11,12]. Until recently, it was unclear if the reading ability of such children fell under the same spectrum as that of children with dyslexia. However, increasing neuroimaging evidence shows that reading disabilities result from the same impairments in phonological processing, regardless of FSIQ. In a previous study using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), both poor readers (PR; children with high FSIQ [dyslexia] and children with low FSIQ, all of whom were native English speakers) exhibited similar patterns of reduced activation in brain regions including the left parietotemporal and occipitotemporal regions during phonological processing [13]. In a different study, children who were all native English speakers with a discrepantly low reading ability relative to their FSIQ showed reduced activation in the left temporoparietal neocortex relative to the control children (typical readers [TR] without FSIQ discrepancy) [14]. In both studies, there was shared neurological atypicality in regions associated with phonological processing in children with poor reading ability that was substantially below the level expected for their FSIQ, regardless of FSIQ [13, 14].

This finding indicates that similar to dyslexia, academic achievement in children with poor reading ability might be lower than that in children with typical reading ability among children with borderline FSIQ. However, the relationship between academic achievement and reading ability in children with borderline FSIQ is unknown. Clarifying the relationship in children with borderline FSIQ between reading ability and academic achievement will benefit the field of special education in the future.

The purpose of the present study was to clarify the characteristics of academic achievement and reading ability in children with lower FSIQ (85 ≥ FSIQ ≥ 70) and poor reading ability, and differentiate these characteristics from those of children with higher FSIQ (FSIQ > 85) and poor reading ability. Unfortunately, up to this point, it has been impossible to evaluate their reading ability correctly because the poor reading ability is also associated with lower FSIQ [15]. The reading ability of children has typically been evaluated using reading data from TDC, contributing to the risks of overdiagnosis of children with lower FSIQ and poor reading ability, and underdiagnosis of gifted children with poor reading ability. Therefore, in this study, we first determined linear criteria on reading ability to separate PR from TR (see below, sections “Statistical analysis” “The classification of PR and TR considering FSIQ”). Furthermore, this study functioned to reveal the differences in clinical features of academic achievement and reading ability between children with lower FSIQ and poor reading ability and children with higher FSIQ and poor reading ability.

Materials and methods

Participants

In this prospective study, we enrolled 126 schoolchildren. All the children were referred to at the National Center Hospital, National Center of Neurology and Psychiatry (NCNP), Japan, between 2017 and 2020, based on teachers’ perceptions of poor written exam results and poor reading out aloud of textbooks compared to other children at school. All children were first-visit patients and had been referred by pediatricians in other hospitals or outpatient clinics, or by teachers in their schools or educational institutes. The age range of children in this study was 6–15 years (from elementary to junior high school). All the children lived and attended public schools in Japan, and had parents who spoke Japanese as their first language.

The children completed the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, Fourth Edition (WISC-IV), based on which their intellectual level was assessed. Reading and writing achievement was determined using their reading and writing scores on the Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children, Second Edition (KABC-II; standard score = 10) [16]. In KABC-II reading and writing scores, children performed the task of reading and writing questions, which involve a mixture of Chinese characters and hiragana (Japanese syllabary). Chinese characters in the questions are arranged in the order children learn at school. The KABC-II is an important tool for diagnoses of children with dyslexia or academic proficiency evaluation because of the order of the questions that take into consideration the progress of grades [9]. We assessed whole writing and reading achievement using the KABC-II in children with poor reading ability, which was brought by poor reading ability.

The caregivers of the children completed the following questionnaires: a Japanese version of the Swanson, Nolan, and Pelham Rating Scale-IV (SNAP-IV) to assess symptoms of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and oppositional defiant disorder (ODD) [17], and the Parent-Interview Autism Spectrum Disorder Rating Scale-Text Revision (PARS-TR) to assess symptoms of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) [18, 19].

The exclusion criteria were: (1) children with mild to profound intellectual disabilities (FSIQ < 70), and (2) children taking drugs or other treatments for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD).

Hiragana (Japanese syllabary) reading task procedure

The framework of the hiragana reading task was similar to that in Takeuchi et al.’s study [9, 20]. Each child was asked to complete four reading tasks that were written in hiragana script: a monomoraic syllable reading task, four-syllable word reading task, four-syllable non-word reading task, and short sentence reading task. This is the standardized reading examination used to diagnose developmental dyslexia in Japan [9]. The average reading time of each task is calculated from the data of TDC, and expansion of the reading time of more than 2SD is judged as an abnormal result [9]. Children with intelligence typical of their age who complete two or more hiragana reading tasks with a reading time greater than 2 standard deviations (SDs) above the average are diagnosed with dyslexia [9]. Hiragana is a Japanese syllabic script that is characterized by highly transparent sound-character correspondence, which can easily be processed in a phonological manner if linguistic processing is not restricted.

Statistical analysis

Statistical analysis was conducted using JMP software (version 9.0.3; SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC, USA). Children in the study were categorized as having either lower or higher FSIQ based on the threshold of an FSIQ score of 85, which was at the level of − 1 SD. Children were also categorized into PR or TR. First, we confirmed the relationship between FSIQ and reading time using simple regression analysis since a previous study demonstrated a linear relationship between FSIQ and reading ability [15]. Second, the reference value of reading time was calculated from the prediction formula, and children who exceeded the reference value in two or more reading tasks were classified as PR based on the diagnosis of dyslexia [9]. Finally, they were categorized into four groups based on FSIQ threshold and reading ability as follows: higher FSIQ and typical reading ability (H-TR), higher FSIQ and poor reading ability (H-PR), lower FSIQ and typical reading ability (L-TR), and lower FSIQ and poor reading ability (L-PR) groups.

We investigated how well the classification of TR and PR in the higher FSIQ group was obtained based on the SD value, which is a standard method for evaluating reading ability, and the reference value from the IQ adjusted- prediction formula, since children with a higher FSIQ were assumed to have normal intelligence.

Pearson’s χ2 test was performed to evaluate the proportional differences between 4 groups for gender and right-handedness. Pearson’s χ2 test was also performed to evaluate the proportional differences between higher FSIQ and lower FSIQ groups for number of children with PR. One-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) was performed to examine group differences in age at examination, three SNAP-IV items (inattention, hyperactivity, and ODD), and PARS-TR scores. Subsequently, post hoc analysis with Tukey’s honestly significant difference (HSD) test was performed.

Associations between reading and writing achievement (reading score and writing score from KABC-II) and age, intelligence, and hiragana reading tasks were assessed using Pearson’s product-moment correlation coefficients for the higher and lower FSIQ groups. In addition, associations between word reading and non-word reading tasks from hiragana reading tasks, and reading and writing achievement scores (reading score and writing score from KABC-II), age, and intelligence were also assessed using Pearson’s product-moment correlation coefficients for the higher and lower FSIQ groups. Statistical significance was set at p < 0.05.

Results

The classification of PR and TR considering FSIQ

Table 1 shows the clinical demographic parameters of the children enrolled in the study (i.e., 126 children identified as having poor academic performance in school).

Table 1 Clinical demographics of children

The simple linear regression analysis revealed a linear relationship between the FSIQ and SD value of reading time for each of the four reading tasks (monomoraic syllable reading task: R2 = 0.052, F = 6.818, p = 0.010, and reference value = 7.421–0.054×FSIQ; four-syllable word reading task: R2 = 0.068, F = 9.024, p = 0.003, and reference value = 9.276–0.073×FSIQ; four-syllable non-word reading task: R2 = 0.067, F = 8.888, p = 0.004, and reference value = 9.387–0.068×FSIQ; short sentence reading task: R2 = 0.040, F = 5.176, p = 0.025, and reference value = 8.085–0.061×FSIQ). The reference values for each FSIQ of every reading task were summarized in additional file 1. Children were divided into PR and TR based on the calculated reference value in their reading time relative to their FSIQ. There were 50, 41, 18, and 17 children in H-TR, H-PR, L-TR, and L-PR groups, respectively.

There was no difference in the proportion of PR between the higher FSIQ and lower FSIQ groups (45.1 % vs. 48.6 %) (p = 0.723). The classification using 2 SDs beyond the average score on reading tasks identified 38 poor readers in the higher FSIQ group, whereas the classification using the prediction formula identified 41 poor readers. Eighty-two out of 91 children (90.1 %) in the higher FSIQ group were identified in the same group from the two types of methods

Children’s background

Table 1 shows the participant’s demographics. There was no difference based on sex, age at examination, or proportion of right-handedness among the four groups divided by IQ and reading ability. There was also no difference in the scores on the three items of SNAP-IV and PARS-TR scores among the four groups.

An additional table shows results of intelligence, reading ability, and reading and writing achievement of PR and TR in both higher and lower FSIQ groups (see Additional file 2).

Factors related to reading and writing achievement and reading ability

Table 2 shows the associations between reading and writing achievement and participant demographics and between reading ability and participant demographics. In the higher FSIQ group, there were stronger positive relationships between reading scores from KABC-II and FSIQ, VCI, and WMI, and a stronger negative relationship between the reading score and each hiragana reading task. Furthermore, there were positive relationships between the writing scores from KABC-II and FSIQ and WMI, and stronger negative relationships between the writing score and monomoraic syllables, four-syllable word reading, and four-syllable non-word reading tasks. In contrast, in the lower FSIQ group, there was a negative relationship between reading and writing scores from KABC-II and age. In addition, there was a strong negative relationship between four-syllable non-word reading time and FSIQ, and VCI in the higher FSIQ group. The lower FSIQ group displayed a strong negative relationship between four-syllable word reading time and age.

Table 2 Associations between reading and writing achievement, reading ability, and intellectual level

Discussion

To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to clarify the clinical characteristics of Japanese children with lower FSIQ (85 ≥ FSIQ ≥ 70) and poor reading ability.

There was no difference in the proportion of PR between higher and lower FSIQ groups. Poor reading and writing achievement from KABC-II in the lower FSIQ group was associated with older age, but not with reading ability or FSIQ. In contrast, poor reading and writing achievement in the higher FSIQ group was associated with low FSIQ and low scores on its subscales, and poor reading ability.

The classification of PR and TR considering FSIQ

In our study, participants were evaluated depending on FSIQ. Similar to previous studies, reading difficulty was evaluated by reading comprehension (IQ) and phonological awareness [10, 21]. Children were diagnosed with dyslexia if their reading ability was much lower than their other cognitive abilities despite having intelligence typical of children their age. In addition, among children with low reading comprehension, some children also had low phonological awareness [10, 11, 21]. In a previous fMRI study, regardless of FSIQ, PR had similar kinds of reading disabilities concerning phonological processing (reduced activation in the left parietotemporal and occipitotemporal regions) [13]. Namely, in PR, the relationship between FSIQ and reading ability, which was lower than that expected based on the obtained FSIQ, was relative. Furthermore, since a previous study revealed that FSIQ has a positive relationship with decoding and visual processing speed, which are significant factors of reading ability, a linear relationship between FSIQ and reading time was presumed [15]. Therefore, it is necessary to set the reading ability threshold according to FSIQ, and our method for classifying children as PR and TR considering FSIQ was valuable, especially for those borderline FSIQ and gifted children, whose evaluation of reading ability by SD value was easily distorted.

Since Japanese children are diagnosed with dyslexia based on reading time > 2 SDs on two or more out of the four hiragana tasks, children were also diagnosed based on the number of tasks above the reference value adjusted by FSIQ in this study. In fact, the reading times of the four tasks were first converted to SD value for the diagnosis of dyslexia. Furthermore, because there is a linear correlation between the FSIQ and SD values, the FSIQ-adjusted reference values were calculated from the raw SD values of hiragana reading tasks. In addition, since diagnoses by 2 SDs above the average value and diagnoses by reference values were the same in 90 % of children with higher FSIQ, the clinical practicality of our new diagnostic method has been confirmed.

In this study, we classified children identified as having low academic performance in school into PR and TR considering FSIQ. The diagnostic tools used for dyslexia (hiragana reading tasks) also helped find out children with lower FSIQ and poor reading ability.

The relationship between reading ability and reading and writing achievement

In our study, the poor reading ability was associated with poor reading and writing achievement in the higher FSIQ group. A previous study demonstrated that the achievement gap caused by poor reading ability between TDC and children with dyslexia appeared as early as the first grade, and persisted not only in reading ability, but also in the verbal components of IQ (i.e., vocabulary, information, comprehension, and similarities subtests; WISC-Revised) [4]. Low levels of literacy achievement at the end of compulsory school education in Finland also significantly increased the likelihood of delayed graduation from upper secondary schools [5]. In particular, even in a study of university students with dyslexia, one-fifth of students had very low academic achievement [6]. As in previous studies, the present study revealed that poor reading ability leads to poor reading and writing achievement in children with higher FSIQ, who were identified as having poor academic performance in school. Importantly, this relationship was revealed only in the higher FSIQ group.

The relationship between age and reading and writing achievement, and between age and reading ability

In the lower FSIQ group, lower reading and writing achievement was associated with older age at examination, but not with reading ability.

As mentioned in section “The relationship between reading ability and reading and writing achievement”, previous studies have shown that poor reading ability in children with dyslexia results in poorer academic achievement than TDC from the first grade onwards [4,5,6]. Our study’s results in the higher FSIQ group are consistent with this finding. Furthermore, in our study, reading and writing achievement scores for those in the lower FSIQ group were lower regardless of reading ability than scores for TR in the higher FSIQ group. In addition, there was no relationship between reading ability and reading and writing achievement in the lower FSIQ group. This finding reveals that poor reading ability in the lower FSIQ group did not cause poor reading and writing achievement, unlike in the higher FSIQ group. The difference in reading and writing achievement between the lower and higher FSIQ groups might have resulted from the stronger deterioration in reading and writing achievement with age in the lower FSIQ than in the higher FSIQ group.

Concerning the relationship between phonological deficits and age, the persistence of phonological deficits in children and adults with dyslexia has been demonstrated in previous longitudinal studies [4, 22, 23]. Furthermore, considering the difference between TDC, children with dyslexia, and children with low FSIQ and poor reading ability in the relationship between chronological age and reading ability, TDC showed linear development, children with dyslexia showed atypical and non-linear development, and children with low IQ and poor reading ability showed linear development (with a developmental delay compared to TDC) [11]. Therefore, the negative relationship between reading ability and age in the PR in the lower FSIQ group might have resulted from developmental delays of the PRs’ phonological deficits.

According to the previous questionnaire studies in Japan [8, 24], the percentage of children with a fundamental reading difficulty in public elementary school classes decreased as they progressed to higher grades, whereas, the percentage of children whose overall Japanese and mathematics learning proficiency was delayed by more than two grades increased as they progressed to higher grades. It was shown that the proportion of children who deviated from children with average academic achievement increased with age, although age-related improvement in reading ability was observed. In other words, the relationship between age and academic achievement was unknown in previous studies, certain people have been identified whose academic performance deteriorates with age.

A previous study revealed that the persistent academic achievement gap had serious consequences for readers with dyslexia, including lower rates of high school graduation, higher levels of unemployment, and lower earnings because of lowered college attainment [4]. In our study, low reading and writing achievement in the lower FSIQ group was found out not only in PR but also in TR (Additional file 2); moreover, age-related deterioration was observed. Therefore, these negative results might lead to more serious outcomes for the education or employment of children in the lower FSIQ group than for PR in the higher FSIQ group.

Unfortunately, background factors of age-related deterioration in academic achievement and reading ability were not clear in this study. However, one of the reasons might be the difficulty in accumulating learning because children with both higher IQ and lower IQ attend the same classes in Japanese public schools, such that children with lower FSIQ have fewer opportunities to receive appropriate educational support than children with higher FSIQ.

Special education for children with lower FSIQ

Concerning the special education of children with lower FSIQ, earlier special support, and independent evaluations of reading ability and reading and writing achievement by teachers may be important because of the lack of a relationship between reading ability and reading and writing achievement in children with lower FSIQ. Furthermore, previous studies revealed that early and evidence-based reading instruction and teacher training offer the potential to reduce and perhaps even close the achievement gap between children with dyslexia and TR and bring their trajectories closer over time [4, 25, 26]. Since PR in the lower FSIQ group were more likely to demonstrate lower reading achievement at an older age, these children need not only special support similar to that offered to PR in the higher FSIQ group but also more careful educational interventions and more frequent evaluations of their reading and writing achievement than PR in the higher FSIQ group.

Limitations

This study had several limitations. First, the number of children analyzed was small, affecting the generalizability of the results. Second, the identification of whether children had poor academic performance in school was based on the subjective judgment of teachers and referral doctors, rather than an objective index. Third, our findings might be specific to Japanese-speaking contexts, and therefore, ungeneralizable to children speaking other languages. Fourth, observation of children’s own adaptive behavior to their learning and educational support could not be considered insufficient. However, there was no difference in the severity of ADHD and ASD symptoms using SNAP-IV and PARS-TR between H-TR, H-PR, L-TR, and L-PR groups in our study. Therefore, there might not be a decrease in adaptive behavior brought by the trait of NDDs, although it was well known that severely autistic children found it more difficult to adapt to overall daily life than TDC in previous studies [27, 28].

Conclusions

In our study, poor reading and writing achievement in the higher FSIQ group was associated with low FSIQ and low scores on its subscales as well as poor reading ability, as also shown in previous studies. However, poor reading and writing achievement was associated with older age, but not with poor reading ability in the lower FSIQ group. Therefore, children with lower FSIQ need appropriate educational interventions based on independent assessments of their reading and writing achievement and reading ability. Moreover, they need more frequent evaluations of their reading and writing achievement than children with higher FSIQ and poor reading ability since they are more likely to demonstrate lower reading achievement at an older age.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets generated and/or analyzed during the current study are not publicly available (study under progress) but are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

FSIQ:

Full-scale intellectual quotient

TDC:

Typically developing children

ANOVA:

Analysis of variance

FMRI:

Functional magnetic resonance imaging

NCNP:

National Center of Neurology and Psychiatry

WISC-IV:

Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, Fourth Edition

KABC-II:

Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children, Second Edition

SNAP-IV:

Swanson, Nolan, and Pelham Rating Scale-IV

ODD:

Oppositional defiant disorder

PARS-TR:

Parent-Interview Autism Spectrum Disorder Rating Scale-Text Revision

ASD:

Autism spectrum disorder

ADHD:

Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder

SD:

Standard deviation

TR:

Typical readers

PR:

Poor readers

HSD:

Honestly significant difference

References

  1. Arnold LE, Hodgkins P, Kahle J, Madhoo M, Kewley G. Long-term outcomes of ADHD: academic achievement and performance. J Atten Disord.2020;24(1):73–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Estes A, Rivera V, Bryan M, Cali P, Dawson G. Discrepancies between academic achievement and intellectual ability in higher-functioning school-aged children with an autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. 2011;41(8):1044–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Jangmo A, Stålhandske A, Chang Z, Chen Q, Almqvist C, Feldman I, et al. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, school performance, and effect of medication. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2019;58(4):423–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Ferrer E, Shaywitz BA, Holahan JM, Marchione KE, Michaels R, Shaywitz SE. Achievement gap in reading is present as early as first grade and persists through adolescence. J Pediatr. 2015;167(5):1121–5 e2.

  5. Holopainen L, Hakkarainen A. Longitudinal effects of reading and/or mathematical difficulties: the role of special education in graduation from upper secondary education. J Learn Disabil. 2019;52(6):456–67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Olofsson Å, Taube K, Ahl A. Academic achievement of university students with dyslexia. Dyslexia. 2015;21(4):338–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. de Beer J, Engels J, Heerkens Y, van der Klink J. Factors influencing work participation of adults with developmental dyslexia: a systematic review. BMC Public Health. 2014;14:77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Kokuritsu Tokushu-Kyoiku Sougou Kenkyujyo [Japanese National Research Institute of Special Education]. Report C-28: Kyouka-gakushuu-ni tokuina-konnan-wo shimesu jidou-seito-no ruikeika-to shidouhou-no kenkyu [Categorisation of Primary School Children with L.D. and a Study on Remediation Methods] (in Japanese). Kanagawa:1996

  9. Inagaki M, Koeda T, Koike H, Wakamiya E, Kaga M. Tokuiteki Hattatu-Shogai shindan Chiryonotameno jissen guideline [Practical guideline for diagnosis and treatment of specific learning disorders] (in Japanese). Tokyo: Shindan to Chiryo-Sha; 2010.

    Google Scholar 

  10. Tunmer W, Greaney K. Defining dyslexia. J. Learn. Disabil. 2010;43(3):229–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Kuppen SE, Goswami U. Developmental trajectories for children with dyslexia and low IQ poor readers. Dev Psychol. 2016;52(5):717–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Kuppen S, Huss M, Goswami U. A longitudinal study of basic auditory processing and phonological skills in children with low IQ. Appl Psycholinguistics. 2013;35(6):1109–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Tanaka H, Black JM, Hulme C, Stanley LM, Kesler SR, Whitfield-Gabrieli S, et al. The brain basis of the phonological deficit in dyslexia is independent of IQ. Psychol Sci. 2011;22(11):1442–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Hancock R, Gabrieli JDE, Hoeft F. Shared temporoparietal dysfunction in dyslexia and typical readers with discrepantly high IQ. Trends Neurosci Educ. 2016;5(4):173–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Tiu RD, Jr., Thompson LA, Lewis BA. The role of IQ in a component model of reading. J Learn Disabil. 2003;36(5):424–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Kumagami T, Kumagai K. Measuring adjustment in Japanese juvenile delinquents with learning disabilities using the Japanese version of Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children II. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2014;68(10):768–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Inoue Y, Ito K, Kita Y, Inagaki M, Kaga M, Swanson JM. Psychometric properties of Japanese version of the Swanson, Nolan, and Pelham, version-IV Scale-Teacher Form: a study of school children in community samples. Brain Dev. 2014;36(8):700–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Kamio Y, Yukihiro R, Adachi J, Ichikawa H, Inoue M, Uchiyama T. Reliability and validity of the pervasive developmental disorder (PDD)-Autism Society Japan Rating Scale (PARS): a behavior checklist for adolescents and adults with PDDs. Seishinigaku. 2006;48:495–505.

    Google Scholar 

  19. Ito H, Tani I, Yukihiro R, Adachi J, Hara K, Ogasawara M, et al. Validation of an interview-based rating scale developed in Japan for pervasive developmental disorders. Res Autism Spectr Disord. 2012;6(4):1265–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Takeuchi A, Koeda T, Takayanagi T, Sato K, Sugino N, Bonno M, et al. Reading difficulty in school-aged very low birth weight infants in Japan. Brain Dev. 2016;38(9):800–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Snowling MJ. Early identification and interventions for dyslexia: a contemporary view. J Res Spec Educ Needs. 2013;13(1):7–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Svensson I, Jacobson C. How persistent are phonological difficulties? A longitudinal study of reading retarded children. Dyslexia. 2006;12(1):3–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Moojen SMP, Gonçalves HA, Bassôa A, Navas AL, de Jou G, Miguel ES. Adults with dyslexia: how can they achieve academic success despite impairments in basic reading and writing abilities? The role of text structure sensitivity as a compensatory skill. Ann Dyslexia. 2020;70(1):115–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Wydell TN. Dyslexia in Japanese and the’hypothesis of granularity and transparency’.2003.

  25. Connor CM, Morrison FJ, Fishman B, Crowe EC, Al Otaiba S, Schatschneider C. A longitudinal cluster-randomized controlled study on the accumulating effects of individualized literacy instruction on students’ reading from first through third grade. Psychol Sci. 2013;24(8):1408–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Ehrhardt J, Huntington N, Molino J, Barbaresi W. Special education and later academic achievement. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2013;34(2):111–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Duncan AW, Bishop SL. Understanding the gap between cognitive abilities and daily living skills in adolescents with autism spectrum disorders with average intelligence. Autism. 2015;19(1):64–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Pugliese CE, Anthony L, Strang JF, Dudley K, Wallace GL, Kenworthy L. Increasing adaptive behavior skill deficits from childhood to adolescence in autism spectrum disorder: role of executive function. J Autism Dev Disord.2015;45(6):1579–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Funding

This work was supported in part by the Meiji Yasuda Mental Health Foundation, Japan (grant number 2020-1-007 to R.U.) and Intramural Research Grant for Neurological and Psychiatric Disorders of NCNP, Japan [2-7 to T.O.].

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

R.U. and Y. Kita decided on the conception and design of the study. Y. Kaga, E.N., T.O., and M.I. jointly carried out the acquisition of data (participant collection and data curation). R.U. and Y. Kita performed the data analysis. R.U. wrote the manuscript and prepared the figure. Y. Kita and T.O. supervised this work. All the authors have approved the final article.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Riyo Ueda.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The study protocol followed the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki. The Institutional Review Board at the National Center of Neurology and Psychiatry approved this study (#A2014-114). In all cases, the parent or parents agreed to their child’s participation in this study and provided informed consent before the study commenced.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1.

The results of the reference value from the IQ adjusted-prediction formula.

Additional file 2.

Examination results for intelligence, reading ability, and reading and writing achievement

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Ueda, R., Kaga, Y., Kita, Y. et al. Clinical profile of reading ability and reading and writing achievement of children with borderline full-scale intellectual quotient: a prospective study. BMC Pediatr 21, 389 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12887-021-02865-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12887-021-02865-z

Keywords