Skip to main content

Neurocognitive functioning in individuals with congenital central hypoventilation syndrome

Abstract

Background

Congenital central hypoventilation syndrome (CCHS) is a rare disorder characterized by respiratory system abnormalities, including alveolar hypoventilation and autonomic nervous system dysregulation. CCHS is associated with compromised brain development and neurocognitive functioning. Studies that evaluate cognitive skills in CCHS are limited, and no study has considered cognitive abilities in conjunction with psychosocial and adaptive functioning. Moreover, the roles of pertinent medical variables such as genetic characteristics are also important to consider in the context of neurocognitive functioning.

Methods

Seven participants with CCHS ranging in age from 1 to 20 years underwent neuropsychological evaluations in a clinic setting.

Results

Neurocognitive testing indicated borderline impaired neurocognitive skills, on average, as well as relative weaknesses in working memory. Important strengths, including good coping skills and relatively strong social skills, may serve as protective factors in this population.

Conclusion

CCHS was associated with poor neurocognitive outcomes, especially with some polyalanine repeat expansion mutations (PARMS) genotype. These findings have important implications for individuals with CCHS as well as medical providers for this population.

Peer Review reports

Background

Congenital central hypoventilation syndrome (CCHS; OMIM #209880) is a rare disorder with an autosomal dominant mode of inheritance, occurring in 1 in 200,000. CCHS typically presents within the neonatal period and is characterized by respiratory system dysregulation, including alveolar hypoventilation with insensitivity to resultant hypoxemia and hypercarbia [1]. CCHS patients often have autonomic nervous system (ANS) dysregulation, including temperature dysregulation, transient abrupt asystoles, severe breath holding spells, altered gut motility, severe constipation, pupillary abnormalities, and decreased perception of pain [2,3,4]. The paired-like homeobox 2B (PHOX2B) gene was identified as the disease-defining gene for CCHS [5]. Due to potential for repeated hypoxemia and hypercarbia among individuals with CCHS, neurocognitive functioning is often impaired [6], and comprehensive neuropsychological assessment (which includes neurocognitive testing as well as consideration of psychosocial functioning and adaptive skills) has been recommended as part of routine medical care among this population [7]. While a few studies have evaluated neural abnormalities in CCHS patients [8,9,10], only a handful of studies have examined neurocognitive functioning [11,12,13,14,15]. The limited available data from these studies demonstrates overall intellectual functioning falling within the borderline impaired to low average range but with substantial variability. However, studies that also consider psychosocial outcomes, including emotional and behavioral symptoms as well as adaptive skills, are limited [8, 12]. In order to better inform medical, psychological, and educational interventions for this population, it is important to characterize these aspects of neuropsychological functioning.

It is also important that these outcomes be considered in the context of genetic information (i.e., presence of polyalanine repeat expansion mutations, or PARMs). In their most recent clinical policy statement, the American Thoracic Society [1] summarized findings regarding the role of PARMs and non-PARMs (NPARMs) in CCHS. In addition to the mutation in the PHOX2B gene, which is required for a diagnosis, over 90% of individuals with CCHS will also be heterozygous for an inframe PARM coding for 24 to 33 alanines in the mutated protein. Genotypic variations are associated with different disease severity. For example, patients with genotypes from 20/27 to 20/33 typically require continuous ventilatory support.

The purpose of the current study is to describe neuropsychological functioning among individuals with CCHS by considering their cognitive skills in concert with their psychosocial and adaptive outcomes and in the context of a relevant medical variable: presence of PARMS vs. NPARMS. We expect that PARMs will be related to poorer neurocognitive outcomes.

Method

Participants

IRB approval was obtained from the institution of the investigators in order to complete a retrospective chart review, and no further permissions were required. We drew the current sample (N = 7) of PHOX2B confirmed CCHS patients from a comprehensive pediatric care clinic housed in a large medical center in the southwestern United States. The clinic provides comprehensive care for patients with acute and chronic conditions, including children with rare pulmonary conditions. The team includes pediatricians, pediatric Pulmonologists, nurse practitioners, neuropsychologists, and social workers.

Participants ranged in age from 12 months to 20 years (M = 7.43 years, SD = 6.55 years), including 5 females and 2 males. Six patients were Hispanic and one was African American. Three of the participants had the 20/25 PARM genotype, one had 20/26 genotype, and three had 20/27 genotype. None of our participants were heterozygous for an NPARM in the PHOX2B gene. All three participants with 20/27 genotype required ventilatory support 24 hours per day. The participant with 20/26 genotype also required support. However, there was variability across the three participants with the 20/25 genotype: one required 24 hour/day ventilation, one needed support only at night, and one did not require support.

Measures

Neuropsychological assessment for CCHS patients occurred in the context of routine medical care, as recommended by the American Thoracic Society [1]. All assessments were administered by a trained graduate student who was supervised by a licensed neuropsychologist. Neurocognitive tests were administered while caregivers completed rating scales of psychosocial and adaptive skills. Due to acute illness, the full testing battery was not administered to one patient.

Neurocognitive functioning, including an estimate of IQ, was obtained with the Wechsler Intelligence Scales for five participants. Three patients were administered the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, Fifth Edition (WISC-V) [16], which is an individual test of general intelligence for children aged 6 to 16. One patient was administered the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence, Fourth Edition (WPPSI-IV) [17], which evaluates cognitive development in children aged 2 to 7 years and one patient was administered the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, Fourth Edition (WAIS-IV) [18], which evaluates cognitive functioning among individuals aged 16 to 90 years. The final patient was administered the Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development, Third Edition (Bayley-III) [19], which evaluates the developmental functioning of infants and children aged 1–42 months.

The Behavioral Assessment System for Children, Second Edition (BASC-2) [20] was used to evaluate psychosocial functioning (i.e., emotional, behavioral symptoms) in six patients. Caregiver ratings of executive functions were obtained with the Behavioral Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF) [21]. Three patients were administered the BRIEF (ages 5 to 18) and two participants were administered the BRIEF, Preschool Edition (BRIEF-P [22]; ages 2 to 5). Adaptive functioning was evaluated with the Adaptive Behavior Assessment Scales, Third Edition (ABAS-3) [23] for six patients.

Data analysis

We provide test results for each subject, including subtest scores and composite index scores, as well as means and standard deviations for each score. Due to our small sample size we do not report results from statistical tests, including correlations. Although we computed correlations among primary index scores, results demonstrated spuriously high correlation coefficients that cannot be interpreted meaningfully due to the small sample. Because of the potential for misleading conclusions about the population-level relationships between these scores, we do not report those results here. Unless specifically noted, results discussed below refer to standard scores (M = 100, SD = 15).

Results

Demographics, medical variables, and IQ scores are summarized in Table 1.

Table 1 Demographics, Medical Variables, and IQ Scores

Relationship between IQ scores and PARMs

On average, intellectual functioning fell in the borderline impaired range (M = 72.33, SD = 22.36). Participants with the 20/27 genotype had, on average, substantially lower IQ (M = 58.33, SD = 18.15) than those with the 20/25 genotype (M = 86.33, 18.23). Among the three participants with the 20/25 genotype, a clear relationship emerged between need for ventilatory support and IQ, such that 24 hour/day support was associated with IQ in the impaired range, partial support (nighttime only) was associated with low average IQ, and no ventilatory support was associated with average IQ.

Wide variability in our sample’s age range made direct comparison between tests difficult; thus, we separated the tests by domain and summarized the general pattern of results, making direct comparisons where possible. All scores are reported by domain in Tables 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 7 and 8.

Table 2 Verbal Outcomes
Table 3 Nonverbal Outcomes
Table 4 Processing Speed Outcomes
Table 5 Working Memory Outcomes
Table 6 Executive Function Outcomes from the BRIEF and BRIEF-P*
Table 7 Behavioral Outcomes from the BASC-2*
Table 8 Adaptive Behavior Outcomes from the ABAS-3

Verbal abilities

Verbal abilities, reported in Table 2, were generally consistent with IQ scores across our sample. The average verbal composite score (available for six participants) fell in the borderline impaired range (M = 78.83, SD = 19.58).

Non-verbal/perceptual abilities

There was wide variability across tests of non-verbal skill and perceptual ability, with results reported in Table 3. Nonverbal/perceptual abilities ranged from borderline impaired to low average.

Processing speed

Processing speed scores (available for four participants), reported in Table 4, were consistent with full scale IQ, on average, and fell in the borderline impaired range (M = 76.0, SD = 37.35).

Executive functions and working memory

On average, working memory performance (available for five participants), reported in Table 5, fell in the impaired range (M = 69.4, SD = 19.87). Comparisons between the working memory, verbal, and non-verbal/perceptual reasoning indices were made for four participants, which demonstrated reduced working memory relative to other skills (in these four patients, working memory composite = 73.0, verbal composite = 83.25, and non-verbal composite = 82.0). Caregiver ratings of executive functions from the BRIEF (available for five patients), reported in Table 6, demonstrated scores within the average range (T-scores for the Global Executive Composite; M = 54.6, SD = 14.32); however, at-risk levels of working memory difficulties were noted.

Psychosocial outcomes

On average, caregiver ratings of psychosocial outcomes fell within the average range; however, at-risk levels of withdrawal were noted. These results are reported in Table 7.

Adaptive abilities

On average, caregiver ratings of adaptive skills were consistent with full-scale IQ scores (M = 75.0, SD = 15.77). These results are reported in Table 8. The Social Composite was higher than the Conceptual and Practical Composites. Within specific subscales, there were relative weaknesses on functional academics and communication and relative strengths in social abilities.

Discussion

Neurocognitive outcomes in CCHS

On average, intellectual functioning was estimated to fall in the borderline impaired range in our sample. This is somewhat lower than prior studies, which estimated IQ to fall within the borderline to low average range [11,12,13,14,15]; however, the wide variability in IQ that we found in our sample is consistent with previous work. Discrepancies in average IQ between our sample and previous work with CCHS patients may be related to small samples sizes across studies.

Working memory emerged as a relative weakness in our sample. This is important and should be explored further in future studies, as working memory is a crucial cognitive process for learning, including reading [24] and math [25]. Moreover, while working memory is considered a component of executive function in many theoretical models [26], we did not employ other executive function tests in this study. It is important to evaluate whether this is a general area of weakness in this population or if there may be a deficit specific for the holding and processing of material in working memory.

Psychosocial outcomes in CCHS

Coping skills, symptoms of emotional difficulties (i.e., anxiety, depression), and behavioral difficulties (i.e., attention problems, impulsivity, etc.) fell within the average range in this sample. This is consistent with findings from Marcus et al. [12] and suggests that psychological wellbeing may be a promising protective factor that can be leveraged in this population. This is also consistent with a study from Ruof et al. [14], which reported behavioral functioning generally falling in the average range despite impairments in intellectual and adaptive functioning. The only at-risk area was the withdrawal subscale, which evaluates the extent to which the individual may avoid others and keep to himself or herself. However, it is possible that this finding was due to circumstances that are secondary to having a complex medical condition, including hospitalizations, numerous doctors appointments, and missing days of school. Overall, findings in this domain are favorable for this population and suggest that individuals with CCHS are resilient and able to cope with their disease effectively.

Adaptive outcomes in CCHS

Adaptive outcomes were consistent with IQ scores in this sample, which was expected since these abilities tend to covary with one another, particularly among individuals with delayed or disordered development [27]. This is consistent with the only prior study that assessed adaptive skills in this population [14]. A relative weakness was noted in communication skills; however, this is likely secondary to ventilatory dependence and need for tracheostomy [28]. Social skills emerged as a relative strength, despite a weakness in communication. Because well-developed social skills are associated with behavioral and adaptive skill development, as well as mental health outcomes [29], we believe this is another protective factor that may help bolster outcomes in this population.

Implications for individuals with CCHS

Findings linking neurocognitive functioning to PARMs have important implications for early identification and treatment of individuals with CCHS. For instance, children with the 20/26 and 20/27 genotypes would likely benefit from early intensive interventions to bolster later cognitive abilities. However, because neurocognitive testing often occurs when the child is a toddler or early school aged, genetic testing can precede this evaluation and provide some insight into the child’s level of risk. Although genetic information can help guide medical care for CCHS patients, our findings support the recommendation from the American Thoracic Society [1] that all individuals with CCHS should receive a comprehensive neuropsychological evaluation to document cognitive strengths and weaknesses in order to inform diagnostic and treatment recommendations.

Limitations and future directions

Our conclusions must be considered in the context of a few limitations, including our small sample. We were unable to perform sophisticated statistical analyses because of our small sample and lack of consistency in tests across participants. It will continue to be difficult for research groups to obtain larger samples of individuals with CCHS. Therefore, it is our recommendation that research groups with access to this population collaborate by utilizing a similar testing battery, compiling databases across labs, and conducting more rigorous statistical analyses with this population.

More in-depth cognitive testing, particularly within the domains of working memory and other executive functions, will be important for future research in order to better understand the specific deficits that are common in this population. Future studies should also consider including academic screening tests (reading and math).

With regard to genetics, more work is needed to further understand the heritability of CCHS. It may be helpful to evaluate cases in which multiple family members across multiple generations have been diagnosed.

Despite these limitations, we believe we have contributed important knowledge to the field’s understanding of CCHS because we are the first to integrate neurocognition, psychosocial skills, adaptive abilities, genetics, and need for ventilatory support.

Conclusions

Our findings support the need for comprehensive neuropsychological evaluation in individuals with CCHS. Genetic testing in infancy should precede neuropsychological testing and may be used to provide preliminary prognostic information about the child’s risk status. Our findings demonstrated a relative weakness in working memory, which should be considered in future studies. Additionally, findings from psychosocial and adaptive evaluation highlight a number of protective factors in this population, including good coping skills and relatively strong social skills.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

ABAS-3:

Adaptive Behavior Assessment Scales, Third Edition

ANS:

Autonomic nervous system

BASC-2:

Behavioral Assessment System for Children, Second Edition

Bayley-III:

Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development, Third Edition

BRIEF:

Behavioral Rating Inventory of Executive Function

CCHS:

Congenital central hypoventilation syndrome

CPAP:

continuous positive airway pressure

IQ:

intelligence quotient

M:

mean

NPARMS:

non-polyalanine repeat expansion mutations

PARMS:

polyalanine repeat expansion mutations

PHOX2B:

Paired-like homeobox 2B

SD:

standard deviation

WAIS-IV:

Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, Fourth Edition

WISC-V:

Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, Fifth Edition

WPPSI-IV:

Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence, Fourth Edition

References

  1. Weese-Mayer DE, Berry-Kravis EM, Ceccherini I, Keens TG, Loghmanee DA, Trang H. An official ATS clinical policy statement: congenital central hypoventilation syndrome: genetic basis, diagnosis, and management. Am J Respir Crit Care Med. 2010 Mar 15;181(6):626–44.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  2. Silvestri JM, Hanna BD, Volgman AS, Jones PJ, Barnes SD, Weese-Mayer DE. Cardiac rhythm disturbances among children with idiopathic congenital central hypoventilation syndrome. Pediatr Pulmonol. 2000 May;29(5):351–8.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  3. Weese-Mayer DE, Shannon DC, Keens TG, Silvestri JM. American Thoracic Society statement. Idiopathic congenital central hypoventilation syndrome. Diagnosis and management. Am J Respir Crit Care Med. 1999;160:368–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Weese-Mayer DE, Silvestri JM, Menzies LJ, Morrow-Kenny AS, Hunt CE, Hauptman SA. Congenital central hypoventilation syndrome: diagnosis, management, and long-term outcome in thirty-two children. J Pediatr. 1992 Mar 1;120(3):381–7.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  5. Matera I, Bachetti T, Puppo F, Di Duca M, Morandi F, Casiraghi GM, Cilio MR, Hennekam R, Hofstra R, Schöber JG, Ravazzolo R. PHOX2B mutations and polyalanine expansions correlate with the severity of the respiratory phenotype and associated symptoms in both congenital and late onset central hypoventilation syndrome. J Med Genet. 2004 May 1;41(5):373–80.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  6. Zelko FA, Nelson MN, Leurgans SE, Berry-Kravis EM, Weese-Mayer DE. Congenital central hypoventilation syndrome: neurocognitive functioning in school age children. Pediatr Pulmonol. 2010 Jan;45(1):92–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Silvestri JM, Weese-Mayer DE, Nelson MN. Neuropsychologic abnormalities in children with congenital central hypoventilation syndrome. J Pediatr. 1992 Mar 1;120(3):388–93.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  8. Macey PM, Richard CA, Kumar R, Woo MA, Ogren JA, Avedissian C, Thompson PM, Harper RM. Hippocampal volume reduction in congenital central hypoventilation syndrome. PLoS One. 2009;4(7).

  9. Kumar R, Ahdout R, Macey PM, Woo MA, Avedissian C, Thompson PM, Harper RM. Reduced caudate nuclei volumes in patients with congenital central hypoventilation syndrome. Neuroscience. 2009 Nov 10;163(4):1373–9.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  10. Kumar R, Macey PM, Woo MA, Harper RM. Selectively diminished corpus callosum fibers in congenital central hypoventilation syndrome. Neuroscience. 2011;178:261–9.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  11. Charnay AJ, Antisdel-Lomaglio JE, Zelko FA, Rand CM, Le M, Gordon SC, Vitez SF, Jennifer WT, Brogadir CD, Nelson MN, Berry-Kravis EM. Congenital central hypoventilation syndrome: neurocognition already reduced in preschool-aged children. Chest. 2016 Mar 1;149(3):809–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Marcus CL, Jansen MT, Poulsen MK, Keens SE, Nield TA, Lipsker LE, Keens TG. Medical and psychosocial outcome of children with congenital central hypoventilation syndrome. J Pediatr. 1991 Dec 1;119(6):888–95.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  13. Weese-Mayer DE, Rand CM, Berry-Kravis EM, Jennings LJ, Loghmanee DA, Patwari PP, Ceccherini I. Congenital central hypoventilation syndrome from past to future: model for translational and transitional autonomic medicine. Pediatr Pulmonol. 2009 Jun;44(6):521–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Ruof H, Hammer J, Tillmann B, Ghelfi D, Weber P. Neuropsychological, behavioral, and adaptive functioning of Swiss children with congenital central hypoventilation syndrome. J Child Neurol. 2008 Nov;23(11):1254–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Zelko FA, Stewart TM, Brogadir CD, Rand CM, Weese-Mayer DE. Congenital central hypoventilation syndrome: broader cognitive deficits revealed by parent controls. Pediatr Pulmonol. 2018 Apr;53(4):492–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Wechsler D. WISC-V: administration and scoring manual. Bloomington, MN: PsychCorp; 2014.

  17. Wechsler D. Wechsler preschool and primary scale of intelligence—fourth edition. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation; 2012.

    Google Scholar 

  18. Wechsler D. Wechsler adult intelligence scale–fourth edition (WAIS–IV). San Antonio, TX: NCS Pearson; 2008.

    Google Scholar 

  19. Bayley N. Bayley scales of infant and toddler development—third edition. Bloomington, MN: PsychCorp; 2006.

  20. Reynolds CR, Kamphaus RW. BASC-2: behavior assessment system for children, second edition manual. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service; 2004.

    Google Scholar 

  21. Gioia GA, Isquith PK, Guy SC, Kenworthy L. BRIEF: Behavior rating inventory of executive function. Psychological Assessment Resources: Odessa, FL; 2000.

  22. Gioia GA, Andrwes K, Isquith PK. Behavior rating inventory of executive function-preschool version (BRIEF-P). Psychological Assessment Resources: Odessa, FL; 1996.

    Google Scholar 

  23. Harrison PL, Oakland T. ABAS-3: adaptive behavior assessment system—third edition. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services; 2015.

    Google Scholar 

  24. Cain K, Oakhill J, Bryant P. Children's reading comprehension ability: concurrent prediction by working memory, verbal ability, and component skills. J Educ Psychol. 2004 Mar;96(1):31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Friso-Van Den Bos I, Van der Ven SH, Kroesbergen EH, Van Luit JE. Working memory and mathematics in primary school children: A metaanalysis. Educational research review. 2013 Dec1 ;10:29–44.

  26. Miyake A, Friedman NP, Emerson MJ, Witzki AH, Howerter A, Wager TD. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cogn Psychol. 2000 Aug 1;41(1):49–100.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  27. Liss M, Harel B, Fein D, Allen D, Dunn M, Feinstein C, Morris R, Waterhouse L, Rapin I. Predictors and correlates of adaptive functioning in children with developmental disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 2001 Apr 1;31(2):219–30.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  28. Hill BP, Singer LT. Speech and language development after infant tracheostomy. J Speech Hear Disord. 1990 Feb;55(1):15–20.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  29. Segrin C, Flora J. Poor social skills are a vulnerability factor in the development of psychosocial problems. Hum Commun Res. 2000 Jul;26(3):489–514.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank the seven patients and their families from our clinic who made this research possible.

Authors‘contributions

KM contributed to idea development, conducted the majority of data collection and aggregation, and drafted the initial manuscript. RM assisted with project conceptualization and data interpretation, reviewed and revised the manuscript, and approved of the final manuscript as submitted. AY assisted with data collection and aggregation, reviewed and revised the manuscript, and approved of the final manuscript as submitted. MCV assisted with data interpretation, reviewed and revised the manuscript, and approved the final manuscript as submitted. HE assisted in data interpretation, reviewed and revised the manuscript, and approved the final manuscript as submitted. KR conceptualized and designed the study, methodological supervision, data collection and aggregation, technical oversight, assisted in drafting the initial manuscript, and approved the final manuscript as submitted. All authors approved the final manuscript as submitted and agree to be accountable for all aspects of the work.

Funding

Not applicable.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Kimberly Rennie.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Internal Review Board (IRB) approval was obtained through the University of Texas Physicians IRB. The study was a retrospective chart review and thus participant consent was not obtained.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

Not applicable.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Macdonald, K.T., Mosquera, R.A., Yadav, A. et al. Neurocognitive functioning in individuals with congenital central hypoventilation syndrome. BMC Pediatr 20, 194 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12887-020-2006-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12887-020-2006-5

Keywords