Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Television viewing and child cognition in a longitudinal birth cohort in Singapore: the role of maternal factors

Abstract

Background

Although infant media exposure has received attention for its implications on child development, upstream risk factors contributing to media exposure have rarely been explored. The study aim was to examine the relationship between maternal risk factors, infant television (TV) viewing, and later child cognition.

Methods

We used a prospective population-based birth cohort study, Growing Up in Singapore Towards healthy Outcomes (GUSTO), with 1247 pregnant mothers recruited in their first trimester. We first explored the relationship of infant TV exposure at 12 months and the composite IQ score at 4.5 years, as measured by the Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test, Second Edition (KBIT-2). Multivariable linear regressions were adjusted for maternal education, maternal mental health, child variables, birth parameters, and other relevant confounders. We then examined the associations of maternal risk factors with the amount of daily TV viewing of 12-month-old infants. Path analysis followed, to test a conceptual model designed a priori to test our hypotheses.

Results

The average amount of TV viewing at 12 months was 2.0 h/day (SD 1.9). TV viewing in hours per day was a significant exposure variable for composite IQ (ß = − 1.55; 95% CI: − 2.81 to − 0.28) and verbal IQ (ß = − 1.77; 95% CI: − 3.22 to − 0.32) at 4.5 years. Our path analysis demonstrated that lower maternal education and worse maternal mood (standardized ß = − 0.27 and 0.14, respectively, p < 0.01 for both variables) were both risk factors for more media exposure. This path analysis also showed that maternal mood and infant TV strongly mediated the relationship between maternal education and child cognition, with an exceptional model fit (CFI > 0.99, AIC 15249.82, RMSEA < 0.001).

Conclusion

Infant TV exposure has a negative association with later cognition. Lower maternal education and suboptimal maternal mental health are risk factors for greater television viewing. Paediatricians have a role in considering and addressing early risks that may encourage television viewing.

Peer Review reports

Background

In the current digital age, children are inevitably exposed to electronic screens regularly and at earlier ages across socioeconomic gradients [1,2,3]. Numerous studies thus far have shown an association between increased screen time and developmental concerns in young children including language delay, externalising behaviours, and executive functioning deficits [4,5,6,7]. A few studies have examined the direct effect of very early screen time on children’s cognition [4, 8,9,10]. Three earlier studies showed mixed results; one found no significant associations between television (TV) viewing in infancy and visual motor cognition at 3 years of age [8] while two other studies showed delayed cognitive skills in children [4, 9]. The only study that looked at infants 12 months and below showed modest adverse effects of TV on cognitive skills at 14 months [10]. However, this study was completed in a low socioeconomic status population and not in a population-based sample.

Another way to understand the mixed evidence produced by previous studies is to consider potential upstream correlates of high media exposure in young children. One likely risk is family socioeconomic status (SES). Both the Family Investment Theory and the Family Stress Theory support low SES as a risk factor [11]. These theories posit that parents from higher SES households have the ability to provide more learning resources and in-person cognitive stimulation, and may not be as reliant on screens [12] whereas parents from lower SES households as a group face frequent stressors, which may disrupt family routines and shared time with their children, resulting in greater screen time.

A rarely explored risk factor for screen time in infants is maternal mental health. There is clear evidence that children of mothers with depressive and anxiety symptoms have poorer developmental outcomes, e.g., externalizing and internalizing behaviours, academics, compared to children of mothers with little or no mood symptoms [13,14,15,16]. Prior research also points to antenatal maternal mood as a stronger correlate of child outcomes than postnatal maternal mood [17, 18]. In fact, our neuroimaging group has shown that antenatal maternal mood alters specific structural brain development of neonates including the amygdala and hippocampus, regardless of postnatal maternal depressive and anxiety symptoms [19, 20]. Little is known, however, about the precise pathways involved in the relationship between maternal mood and developmental outcomes. We hypothesise that media may play a role as an underlying pathway.

Given that the majority of infants are exposed to media use nowadays, [10] exploration of its effect on cognitive skills at this early age is warranted. As infants grow and thrive with responsive interactions and nurturance in their environment for development, it is imperative to study maternal factors in the context of infant media consumption. The aim of this study was to first establish the relationship between TV viewing in infancy and later child cognition in a population-based sample, and secondly, to identify early maternal risk factors for higher screen time. We hypothesize that lower antenatal mood and maternal education are both risks for increased infant TV viewing and for poorer cognitive outcomes. The findings of this study may be leveraged for future interventions, particularly on upstream correlates of negative child outcomes.

Methods

Subjects

This is a prospective population-based cohort study with data obtained from the Growing Up in Singapore Towards healthy Outcomes (GUSTO) study [21]. Pregnant women in their first trimester (N = 1247) were recruited from two large public hospitals in Singapore, the Kandang Kerbau Women’s and Children’s Hospital and the National University Hospital between June 2009 and September 2010. These mothers belonged to one of the three major ethnicities in Singapore (i.e., Chinese, Malay or Indian). At study baseline, 55.9% of the cohort were Chinese, 26.1% Malay and 18.0% Indian. Study participants were followed after delivery as mother-child dyads. A subset of 423 returned at 4.5 years of age for a battery of neurocognitive tests, including the Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test, Second Edition (KBIT-2), as described below. Assessments on children were completed only in English as the school system in Singapore uses English based bilingual education curriculum. For caregivers whose primary language was not English, back-translated questionnaires were provided. We obtained license agreements with the publishers of copy-righted materials for translation into the local languages. The study was approved by the hospitals’ Institutional Review Board.

Study measures

Maternal mood was assessed through 3 questionnaires administered to all mothers between 26 and 28 weeks of pregnancy: the Spielberger State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI), the Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale (EPDS) and the Beck Depression Inventory, Second Edition (BDI-2). The STAI is a well-studied 40-item measure of state and trait anxiety with a 4 point Likert scale in each question [22].The EPDS is a well-validated measure of depression with 10 items on common depressive symptoms [23]. Similarly, the BDI contains 21 items as a measure of depression [24]. Our group conducted a factor analysis of all the questions in these 3 scales and derived a maternal General Negative Mood Factor [25]. This factor was used as a measure of antenatal maternal mood in this study. Higher values on this General Negative Mood Factor denoted worse maternal mood.

TV viewing was measured through a questionnaire completed by parents in 2010 when the children were 12 months of age. Tablets with touch interface were introduced that year and 97% of families were using TV alone as the main source of screen time for children [26]. Parents reported the amount of TV viewed on weekdays and weekends within the past month. In addition, this questionnaire also asked parents about literacy stimulation activities, for example, reading activities with various caregivers, the presence and frequency of bedtime reading, and the amount of books present at home.

As antenatal maternal mood was included as maternal factors, we used antenatal maternal education as an indicator of SES. Maternal education was categorised into a dichotomous variable with university and above as a group and below university as the other. Variables pertaining to pregnancy and delivery, such as gestational age, birthweight, need for resuscitation, and breastfeeding practices, were systematically collected in this cohort and were adjusted for in the analyses. Children also underwent the Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development, Third Edition, at 24 months of age as part of the cohort measures.

The main outcome measure was the Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test, Second Edition (KBIT-2) administered at 4.5 years, a measure of abbreviated intelligence for children and adults aged 4 years to 90 years of age. The KBIT-2 has been shown to have strong correlations with the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, Fourth Edition (WISC-IV), with a correlation coefficient of 0.84 for the Composite IQ [27]. The verbal components of the KBIT-2 consisted of Verbal Knowledge and Riddles, which measured crystallised (i.e., previously learned) abilities, while the nonverbal component, namely Matrices, measured fluid reasoning. Both the composite IQ and the verbal IQ were examined as separate outcome measures.

Analyses

We conducted data analysis using IBM SPSS Statistics, Version 22 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL) and Mplus (Muthen & Muthen, Los Angeles, CA) [28, 29]. We completed linear regression models using TV viewing in hours per day as the independent variable and child IQ at 4.5 years of age as the dependent variable. We first adjusted for maternal and pregnancy-related variables, then subsequently for child-related variables. A final model was then performed which included all covariates with p-value cut-off < 0.1 in the initial models.

The relationships between maternal education, antenatal maternal mood, TV viewing, and cognitive outcomes, were examined using path analysis. Path analysis tested our conceptual model (Fig. 1a) and examined whether TV viewing mediated the effects of maternal variables on child composite IQ. Path analysis was chosen to account for the inter-correlated variables in the model as opposed to simple mediation models. The goodness-of-fit of the entire model was evaluated. Four goodness-of-fit indices were examined to determine how well the model reproduced characteristics of the observed data: Comparative fit index (CFI), Akaike’s Information Criterion (AIC), Bayes Information Criterion (BIC), and root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA). CFI and TFI values > 0.95, RMSEA values of ≤0.05, and lower AIC and BIC in the most parsimonious model indicated a good fit [30]. Missing data were addressed using Maximum Likelihood (ML) Estimation in Mplus version 8 [29]

Fig. 1
figure 1

a Conceptual model for path analysis. b Path Analysis data

.

Results

Complete data for all the variables were available for 387 subjects. The average amount of TV viewing at 12 months of age was 2.0 h/day (SD 1.9). Demographic data are as shown in Table 1. We compared children who were part of this current study against the entire GUSTO cohort and found no significant differences in the demographic variables and maternal mood indices on t-tests and chi-square tests. Consistent with previous data from this cohort, [31] there was a significant difference in TV viewing among the 3 ethnic groups with children of Malay ethnicity having more TV viewing compared to those of Chinese or Indian ethnicity (One-way ANOVA F = 9.07, p < 0.001).

Table 1 Demographic information and descriptive statistics of mothers and children

Univariate analyses showed that TV viewing in hours/day was a significant predictor of child composite IQ score at 4.5 years of age (ß = -2.72, p = < 0.001, 95% CI: − 3.82 to − 1.63). Tables 2 and 3 show the multivariable linear regression results. TV viewing as a linear variable (ß = − 1.55, p = 0.02, 95% CI: − 2.81 to − 0.28) and maternal education (ß = 4.78, p = 0.04, 95% CI: 0.21 to 9.35) were both significant predictors of composite IQ and verbal IQ at 4.5 years of age. In the final regression model, for every extra hour/day of TV watched, composite IQ decreased by 1.55 standard score points. For example, in a 12-month-old infant who watches 3 more hours/day of TV, the IQ would decrease by 4.5 points in standard scores, which is nearly one-thirds of a standard deviation in the normed sample.

Table 2 Linear regression models predicting for composite KBIT score at 4.5 years of age
Table 3 Linear regression models predicting for verbal KBIT score at 4.5 years of age

We also completed a separate logistic regression analysis with amount of 12-month TV dichotomised to > 1 h/day and ≤ 1 h/day which showed that the odds of having a composite IQ score < 70 (i.e. less than 2 SD below mean) was 6.2 times higher (95% CI: 1.4 to 27.7) among children who watched > 1 h/day of TV compared to those who watched less than that amount. IQ scores less than 70 meets the IQ threshold for intellectual disability and hence were chosen as the cut-off.

Our path analysis examining the conceptual model (Fig. 1a) demonstrated that lower maternal education and worse maternal mood (standardised direct coefficient − 0.27 and 0.14, respectively, p < 0.01) were both risk factors for more TV viewing. There was a serial multiple mediation effect of antenatal maternal mood and amount of TV viewing on the relationship between maternal education and child cognition (Fig. 1b). The indirect pathway through maternal mood alone accounted for 26% of the total effect and the indirect pathways involving TV viewing accounted for 7.9% of the total effect between maternal education and child cognition. The model fit was exceptional with a CFI of > 0.99, AIC of 15,249.82, BIC of 15,310.56 and RMSEA of < 0.001.

Discussion

Infant television viewing at 12 months of age is negatively associated with cognitive skills at 4.5 years of age. This association remains even after correction for perinatal, child, and family variables. Moving more upstream, our findings demonstrate that lower maternal education and poorer maternal mood are risk factors for greater media exposure in infancy. The pathways through antenatal maternal mood and infant TV viewing strongly mediated one-thirds of the total effect of maternal education on later child cognition.

Consistent with more recent studies on screen time in infants and toddlers, we showed detrimental effects of TV viewing at 12 months on later cognition [4, 9, 10]. Previously published data showed that for every extra hour/day of TV, decreases in working memory, word recognition, and reading comprehension scores were found (i.e., 0.1, 0.3, and 0.6 points, respectively) [9]. Our finding may be specific to our particular culture and population; nonetheless, together with the mounting evidence from other recent studies, it underscores the deleterious effects and the need for guidelines on infant TV adapted to each country. Our findings also reflect poor adherence to the existing guidelines on TV viewing in infants. [32]

The importance of SES on development and cognition has been well established [33, 34]. The underlying mechanisms for this, although not fully elucidated, include home environment and cognitive stimulation [35]. As such, our finding that maternal education is a strong correlate of cognition is not new; however, the finding that TV viewing mediates this relationship is unique. Mediators are good leverage points of intervention and the amount of TV viewing is a modifiable lifestyle change. Although literacy stimulation is not the main subject of this study, we have also shown here that literacy stimulation as measured through bedtime reading is a positive correlate of composite and verbal IQ. This simple, low-cost activity may thus be another potential intervention target to promote cognitive skills.

In line with prior studies, we demonstrate that worse maternal mood is associated with poorer child cognition. This study adds to literature by elucidating one underlying pathway directly and indirectly through TV viewing. Interestingly, poorer antenatal maternal mood has a stronger association with verbal IQ compared to the composite IQ score, which suggests its importance in language and/or crystallized intelligence. It is possible that changes in the brain of these children in utero may affect structures implicated in language pathways. Conversely, antenatal mood may simply reflect postnatal mood. Crystallized literacy knowledge requires caregiver’s interactions with the child, which are likely impaired in mothers with suboptimal mood. Moreover, it is likely that TV viewing acts only as a proxy for reduced direct engagement with the infant. It is important to note that the direct negative effect of maternal mood on child cognition is greater than that through TV viewing, highlighting that media is but one of the pathways in this relationship between maternal mood and child outcomes.

The above results urge medical professionals to actively screen for early family risk factors, namely low family SES and maternal mental health, and to provide anticipatory guidance around infant TV viewing. Addressing these risk factors in a more targeted fashion will ensure that the high-risk groups receive such recommendations.

The limitations of this study include firstly that screen time is limited to TV and does not consider handheld devices and other forms of media. However, at the time of this study, other devices were not in mainstream use. Nonetheless, future studies will encapsulate all other forms of digital media, which has since been collected in the study cohort. Secondly, we did not account for the nature of content viewed on TV. Evidence in older children aged beyond 36 months suggests that educational content and pro-social shows can have positive effects on the child. [36] However, 12-month-old infants have limited ability to process two-dimensional information through the screen regardless of content. Our final cohort size for this study is moderate as opposed to other cohorts examining screen time in children, yet this study is justifiable because maternal and early factors were explored in addition to the impact of screen time on very young children.

Conclusion

In conclusion, this study confirms the negative relationship between the amount of TV viewing in infancy and cognition in childhood. Lower maternal education and poorer maternal mental health are upstream risk factors for greater TV viewing. This raises important policy implications in terms of identification of specific group of infants who are especially at risk for negative cognitive effects from excessive screen time. It is imperative that paediatricians assess patients for media exposure, especially children from more disadvantaged families and those with mothers facing mental health issues. Parental awareness across the whole population should also be actively encouraged by paediatric and early childhood professionals throughout the community.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets pertaining to this submitted manuscript are available upon request from the authors.

Abbreviations

BDI:

Beck Depression Inventory

EPDS:

Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale

GUSTO:

Growing Up in Singapore Towards healthy Outcomes

KBIT:

Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test

SES:

Socioeconomic status

STAI:

State-Trait Anxiety Inventory

TV:

Television

References

  1. Kabali HK, Irigoyen MM, Nunez-Davis R, Budacki JG, Mohanty SH, Leister KP, et al. Exposure and use of Mobile media devices by young children. Pediatrics. 2015;136(6):1044–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Goh SN, Teh LH, Tay WR, Anantharaman S, van Dam RM, Tan CS, et al. Sociodemographic, home environment and parental influences on total and device-specific screen viewing in children aged 2 years and below: an observational study. BMJ Open. 2016;6(1):e009113.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Cheng S, Maeda T, Yoichi S, Yamagata Z, Tomiwa K. Japan Children's Study Group. Early Television Exposure and Children's Behavioral and Social Outcomes at Age 30 Months. Journal of Epidemiology. 2010;20(Supplement_II):S482–S9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Lin LY, Cherng RJ, Chen YJ, Chen YJ, Yang HM. Effects of television exposure on developmental skills among young children. Infant Behav Dev. 2015;38:20–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Munzer TG, Miller AL, Peterson KE, Brophy-Herb HE, Horodynski MA, Contreras D, et al. Media exposure in low-income preschool-aged children is associated with multiple measures of self-regulatory behavior. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2018;39(4):303–9.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Parkes A, Sweeting H, Wight D, Henderson M. Do television and electronic games predict children's psychosocial adjustment? Longitudinal research using the UK millennium cohort study. Arch Dis Child. 2013;98(5):341–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Linebarger DL, Barr R, Lapierre MA, Piotrowski JT. Associations between parenting, media use, cumulative risk, and Children's executive functioning. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2014;35(6):367–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Schmidt ME, Rich M, Rifas-Shiman SL, Oken E, Taveras EM. Television viewing in infancy and child cognition at 3 years of age in a US cohort. Pediatrics. 2009;123(3):e370–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Zimmerman FJ, Christakis DA. Children’s television viewing and cognitive outcomes: a longitudinal analysis of national data. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2005;159(7):619–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Tomopoulos S, Dreyer BP, Berkule S, Fierman AH, Brockmeyer C, Mendelsohn AL. Infant media exposure and toddler development. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2010;164(12):1105–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Smith JR, Brooks-Gunn J, Klebanov PK. Consequences of living in poverty for young children’s cognitive and verbal ability and early school achievement. Consequences of growing up poor. 1997:132–89.

  12. McLoyd VC. Socioeconomic disadvantage and child development. Am Psychol. 1998;53(2):185–204.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  13. Bergman K, Sarkar P, O'Connor TG, Modi N, Glover V. Maternal stress during pregnancy predicts cognitive ability and fearfulness in infancy. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2007;46(11):1454–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Keim SA, Daniels JL, Dole N, Herring AH, Siega-Riz AM, Scheidt PC. A prospective study of maternal anxiety, perceived stress, and depressive symptoms in relation to infant cognitive development. Early Hum Dev. 2011;87(5):373–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. van Batenburg-Eddes T, de Groot L, Huizink AC, Steegers EA, Hofman A, Jaddoe VW, et al. Maternal symptoms of anxiety during pregnancy affect infant neuromotor development: the generation R study. Dev Neuropsychol. 2009;34(4):476–93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Feldman R, Granat A, Pariente C, Kanety H, Kuint J, Gilboa-Schechtman E. Maternal depression and anxiety across the postpartum year and infant social engagement, fear regulation, and stress reactivity. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2009;48(9):919–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Perren S, von Wyl A, Bürgin D, Simoni H, von Klitzing K. Depressive symptoms and psychosocial stress across the transition to parenthood: associations with parental psychopathology and child difficulty. J Psychosom Obstet Gynecol. 2009;26(3):173–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Huizink AC, Robles De Medina PG, Mulder EJH, Visser GHA, Buitelaar JK. Psychological measures of prenatal stress as predictors of infant temperament. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2002;41(9):1078–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Qiu A, Rifkin-Graboi A, Chen H, Chong YS, Kwek K, Gluckman PD, et al. Maternal anxiety and infants' hippocampal development: timing matters. Transl Psychiatry. 2013;3:e306.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  20. Rifkin-Graboi A, Bai J, Chen H, Hameed WB, Sim LW, Tint MT, et al. Prenatal maternal depression associates with microstructure of right amygdala in neonates at birth. Biol Psychiatry. 2013;74(11):837–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Soh SE, Tint MT, Gluckman PD, Godfrey KM, Rifkin-Graboi A, Chan YH, et al. Cohort profile: growing up in Singapore towards healthy outcomes (GUSTO) birth cohort study. Int J Epidemiol. 2014;43(5):1401–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Spielberger CD. State-trait anxiety inventory: Wiley online library; 2010.

  23. Beck AT, Ward CH, Mendelson M, Mock J, ERBAUGH J. An inventory for measuring depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1961;4(6):561–71.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  24. Cox JL, Holden JM, Sagovsky R. Detection of postnatal depression. Development of the 10-item Edinburgh postnatal depression scale. Br J Psychiatry. 1987;150(6):782–6.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  25. Phua DY, Kee M, Koh DXP, Rifkin-Graboi A, Daniels M, Chen H, et al. Positive maternal mental health during pregnancy associated with specific forms of adaptive development in early childhood: evidence from a longitudinal study. Dev Psychopathol. 2017;29(5):1573–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Zickuhr K. Tablet ownership 2013. Tablet. 2013;19.

  27. Kaufman AS, Kaufman NL. Kaufman brief intelligence test: Wiley online library; 2004.

    Google Scholar 

  28. Corp I. IBM SPSS statistics for windows, version 22.0. IBM Corp Armonk, NY; 2011.

  29. Muthén LK, Muthén BO. Statistical analysis with latent variables. Mplus User’s guide. 1998;2012.

  30. Hu L-T, Bentler PM. Evaluating model fit; 1995.

    Google Scholar 

  31. Bernard JY, Padmapriya N, Chen B, Cai S, Tan KH, Yap F, et al. Predictors of screen viewing time in young Singaporean children: the GUSTO cohort. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2017;14(1):112.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Council On Communications and Media A. media and Young Minds. Pediatrics. 2016;138(5).

  33. Bradley RH, Corwyn RF. Socioeconomic status and child development. Annu Rev Psychol. 2002;53(1):371–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Noble KG, Norman MF, Farah MJ. Neurocognitive correlates of socioeconomic status in kindergarten children. Dev Sci. 2005;8(1):74–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Farah MJ, Betancourt L, Shera DM, Savage JH, Giannetta JM, Brodsky NL, et al. Environmental stimulation, parental nurturance and cognitive development in humans. Dev Sci. 2008;11(5):793–801.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Christakis DA, Garrison MM, Herrenkohl T, Haggerty K, Rivara FP, Zhou C, et al. Modifying media content for preschool children: a randomized controlled trial. Pediatrics. 2013;131(3):431–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank the contribution of the GUSTO study participants. The GUSTO study group includes Allan Sheppard, Amutha Chinnadurai, Anne Eng Neo Goh, Anqi Qiu, Arijit Biswas, Bee Wah Lee, Boon Long Quah, Borys Shuter, Chai Kiat Chng, Cheryl Ngo, Choon Looi Bong, Christiani Jeyakumar Henry, Claudia Chi, Cornelia Yin Ing Chee, Yam Thiam Daniel Goh, Doris Fok, E Shyong Tai, Elaine Tham, Elaine Quah Phaik Ling, Evelyn Xiu Ling Loo, Falk Mueller-Riemenschneider, George Seow Heong Yeo, Heng Hao Tan, Hugo P S van Bever, Iliana Magiati, Inez Bik Yun Wong, Ivy Yee-Man Lau, Izzuddin Bin Mohd Aris, Jeevesh Kapur, Jenny L. Richmond, Jerry Kok Yen Chan, Joanna D. Holbrook, Joanne Yoong, Joao N. Ferreira., Jonathan Tze Liang Choo, Jonathan Y. Bernard, Joshua J. Gooley, Keith M. Godfrey, Kenneth Kwek, Kok Hian Tan, Krishnamoorthy Niduvaje, Kuan Jin Lee, Leher Singh, Lieng Hsi Ling, Lin Lin Su, Ling-Wei Chen, Marielle V. Fortier, Mark Hanson, Mary Foong-Fong Chong, Mary Rauff, Mei Chien Chua, Melvin Khee-Shing Leow, Mya Thway Tint, Neerja Karnani, Ngee Lek, Oon Hoe Teoh, P. C. Wong, Paulin Tay Straughan, Peter D. Gluckman, Pratibha Agarwal, Queenie Ling Jun Li, Rob M. van Dam, Salome A. Rebello, Seang-Mei Saw, See Ling Loy, S. Sendhil Velan, Seng Bin Ang, Shang Chee Chong, Sharon Ng, Shu-E Soh, Sok Bee Lim, Stella Tsotsi, Chin-Ying Stephen Hsu, Sue Anne Toh, Swee Chye Quek, Victor Samuel Rajadurai, Walter Stunkel, Wayne Cutfield, Wee Meng Han, Wei Wei Pang, Yin Bun Cheung, Yiong Huak Chan and Yung Seng Lee.

Funding

This research is funded by the Singapore National Research Foundation under its Translational and Clinical Research (TCR) Flagship Programme of the Singapore Ministry of Health’s National Medical Research Council (NMRC), Singapore (NMRC/TCR/004-NUS/2008; NMRC/TCR/012-NUHS/2014) and by the Institute for Clinical Sciences, Agency for Science Technology and Research (A*STAR), Singapore. The funding body did not have any direct role in the design of the study and collection, analysis, and interpretation of data and in writing this manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

RA and EL conceptualized and designed the study, carried out the analyses, drafted the initial manuscript, and reviewed and revised the manuscript. SC, HYC, DYP, BFPB, LMD and YSC designed the data collection instruments, coordinated and supervised data collection, and critically reviewed the manuscript. LPS, FY, SYC and MJM conceptualised and designed the study, reviewed the analyses and critically reviewed the manuscript. All authors approved the final manuscript as submitted and agree to be accountable for all aspects of the work.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ramkumar Aishworiya.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The study was approved by the hospitals’ Institutional Review Board – the National Healthcare Group Domain Specific Research Board and the Centralised Institutional Review Board of SingHealth Hospitals.

Consent for publication

Not Applicable.

Competing interests

All authors do not have competing interests relevant to this article. Outside of this submitted work, Prof YS Chong, Prof LP Shek, and A/Prof SY Chan as part of the Epigen Academic Consortium, have received research funding from Abbot Nutrition, Nestec, and Danone.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Aishworiya, R., Cai, S., Chen, H.Y. et al. Television viewing and child cognition in a longitudinal birth cohort in Singapore: the role of maternal factors. BMC Pediatr 19, 286 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12887-019-1651-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12887-019-1651-z

Keywords