Skip to main content

Prenatal alcohol exposure and infant gross motor development: a prospective cohort study

A Correction to this article was published on 04 July 2019

This article has been updated

Abstract

Background

Maternal alcohol consumption in pregnancy may have adverse effects on child gross motor (GM) development. There have been few human studies on this topic, particularly ones examining low exposure. This study examined the association between prenatal alcohol exposure (PAE) and infant GM development at 12-months of age.

Methods

Participants were 1324 women recruited from antenatal clinics in Sydney and Perth, Australia. Maternal and paternal alcohol use was assessed in pregnancy via interview; offspring GM development was measured at 12-months with the Bayley Scales of Infant Development (BSID-III).

Results

Any alcohol use in pregnancy was common: 56.1%, of pregnant women drank early in Trimester one (0–6 weeks), however this reduced to 27.9% on average thereafter and at predominantly low levels. However, infant BSID GM scale scores were not found to differ significantly as a function of PAE in the first 6-weeks (low, moderate, binge or heavy PAE), nor with low PAE across pregnancy.

Conclusions

We found no evidence to suggest that low PAE is associated with measurable impairment in infant GM development at 12-months. Further research is needed to examine potential PAE impacts on GM development in heavier exposure groups and through the childhood years when subtle GM deficits may be more detectable.

Peer Review reports

Background

Prenatal alcohol exposure (PAE) has been associated with impairments in infant motor development [1, 2]. Infants diagnosed with Fetal Alcohol Spectrum Disorders (FASD) may exhibit a range of motor impairments [3], including orthopedic and structural deficits, tapering of the distal phalanges, decreased elbow pronation/supination, clubfoot and hand tremors [4,5,6,7,8]. Neuroimaging studies have identified damage to specific regions of the brain among individuals with PAE or FASD [9,10,11,12,13]. In animal studies, PAE has been associated with impaired spinal and peripheral nerve myelination [14, 15], and with reduced motor coordination, speed, response, reflexes, activity, and tone [16, 17].

A number of systematic reviews have examined the relationship between PAE and motor skills. A 2011 review found that high levels of PAE (10 to 30 drinks per week) were associated with impaired offspring motor function [1]. The review did not examine whether specific types of motor skills were more likely to be affected; nor whether gross motor (GM) skills (i.e., coordination of movement using the large muscles of the body) were affected independently of fine motor (FM) skills (i.e., precise, coordinated movements).

A recent review found that neither mild (≤3 drinks per week), moderate (≤6 drinks per week, including some women who drank at least 3 drinks per week), nor binge (≥4 or ≥ 5 drinks per occasion) PAE were significantly associated with motor impairment in children below 5 years of age [18]. Again, however, this study did not differentiate GM from FM skills. Distinguishing these skills and their associations with PAE is important because management strategies to address GM deficits differ considerably from FM interventions [19].

In 2014 Lucas et al. [2] published a systematic review and meta-analysis of GM impairment in children (mean age 3 days to 13 years) diagnosed with FASD or exposed to moderate (2 to ≥14 drinks per week) to heavy (> 10 to 28 drinks per week) or binge (≥5 drinks per occasion) PAE. Results indicated that children with FASD were three times more likely to have a GM impairment, yet moderate to heavy and binge PAE were not significantly associated with GM impairment. Notably, this review did not examine low PAE compared with no PAE [2]. Evidence for GM impairment has not yet been established following only low PAE [20, 21].

This study aimed to examine GM skills in a cohort of 1324 infants (mean age = 12.20 months) from a longitudinal pregnancy cohort with multi-wave data on both the timing and dose of PAE. Uniquely, PAE was assessed at four time points through pregnancy: Trimester one 0–6 weeks (T1a); Trimester one 7–12 weeks (T1b); Trimester two, 13–27 weeks (T2); Trimester three, 28 weeks until birth (T3). GM skills were assessed using the Bayley Scales of Infant Development (BSID-III) [22] at 12-months infant age. Specifically, the aims of the study were threefold: (1) examine the frequency and quantity of maternal alcohol use across pregnancy; (2) examine maternal, infant and partner characteristics associated with PAE in pregnancy compared to abstainers; and, (3) determine whether GM skills were impaired among infants after PAE compared with infants of abstainers, accounting for potential maternal, paternal and infant confounders.

Methods

Participants

Data were from the Triple B Pregnancy Cohort Study, a prospective Australian study of 1634 pregnant women recruited in 2009–13 at antenatal clinics in NSW (n = 1305) and WA (n = 318) [23]. Ethical approval was granted by university and hospital Human Research Ethics Committees. Eligibility criteria included: pregnancy; aged ≥16 years; no major medical complications (mother/fetus); mother/both parents the primary caregiver/s; mentally able to complete assessments; English literary; and informed consent.

Data were collected in pregnancy at T1a, T1b, T2, T3, and at 8-weeks, and 12-months post-birth. By separating T1 into two periods the effects of early PAE could be examined. Most women reported pregnancy awareness around 5–6 weeks gestation [24]; as such, a six-week split broadly represents pre- and post-pregnancy awareness. Of the participants with GM outcome data, we excluded women with inconsistent drinking behaviour (n = 141) and who had infants with very low birth weight (< 1.6 kg; n = 1). For women who gave birth to twins/triplets (n = 34), one child was selected at random for analysis. The final sample comprised 1324 participants (755 with participating partners).

Measures

Study measures are described in Table 1.

Table 1 Summary of Exposure, Outcome and Other Background Confounding and Precision Measures

Alcohol use

Maternal drinking was assessed via interview. Self-reported frequency and quantity (10 g of alcohol per standard drink) of typical use during each trimester, and occasions when women drank more, were recorded. Alcohol use during T3 was assessed retrospectively at the 8-week interview so that consumption across the trimester was captured. Average weekly alcohol consumption was calculated using O’Leary et al.’s PAE categories: abstinent, low, moderate, binge and heavy [25]. A subsample of 85 participants was randomly selected for urine analysis in T3 to confirm self-reported illicit substance use. Agreement between self-reported substance use and urine analysis was 97%, indicating that the information provided via interview was reliable.

Infant GM development

The BSID-III was administered to children at 12-months (mean age = 12.20 months, SD = 0.86, range = 8 to 22 months) by qualified assessors in participants’ homes [22]. Inter-rater reliability in a randomly selected sub-sample was high (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.99; n = 27). Infant scores on the BSID-III were age adjusted for prematurity.

Potential confounders

Maternal socio-demographic background factors included: maternal age at birth; education; birth country; single parent status; Aboriginal and Torres Strait Islander descent; language and household socio-economic status (SES).

Precision variables: To further isolate a causal role for any observed association between PAE and offspring GM development, we systematically entered a range of other possible determinants into the multivariate models. These included: maternal substance use, physical and mental health in pregnancy (tobacco/illicit drug use; depression, anxiety and stress [26, 27]; spousal abuse [28]), estimated IQ [29], parity, pre-pregnancy body mass index (BMI), pregnancy planning, and infant sex and birth outcomes (prematurity, birthweight, head circumference, Apgar score). Potential partner-related confounding factors assessed at T3 were also entered in a series of supplementary analyses. Partner data were available for 57% (n = 754) of participating mothers.

Planned analyses

Analyses were conducted using STATA 14 and SPSS 20 [30, 31]. Missing data was accounted for using multiple imputation [32, 33]. There were four stages of analysis. First, we described maternal drinking patterns (low, moderate, binge and heavy drinking versus abstinence) at all pregnancy timepoints (T1a, T1b, T2 and T3), and partner drinking patterns at T3. Second, binary logistic regression models estimated the association between mother, infant and partner-related factors and PAE (i.e., any drinking in pregnancy versus abstinence).

In the third stage, logistic regression analyses examined the relationship between PAE (T1a, T1b, T2 and T3) and infant GM outcomes at 12-months, controlling for background socio-demographics, and other potential maternal, infant and partner confounders. At T1a all PAE categories were examined. However, due to the low frequency of moderate, binge and heavy drinking in the sample for T1b, T2 and T3 (see Table 2), only low PAE was examined. The reference category was abstinence. The primary outcome was the BSID-III scaled GM score. The unadjusted results were examined first, followed by increasing levels of adjustment for potential confounders. The adjusted analyses included the maternal socio-demographic background factors, followed by factors found to be associated with maternal drinking at the univariate level (p < .10) [34].

Table 2 Patterns of Alcohol Use by Mothers across Pregnancy (N = 1324)

Finally we examined whether the effect of PAE may differ according to an individual’s risk of being exposed to alcohol based on their baseline characteristics [35]. To do this we calculated the propensity of a woman to consume alcohol at low-levels. We then stratified the sample into groups indicating higher and lower risk of low-level alcohol exposure based on their propensity score and compared infant GM outcomes between these groups (see Additional file 2: Section B for a detailed description).

Results

Patterns of alcohol use by mothers across pregnancy

Most women reported alcohol use at some point in pregnancy (61.2%; Table 2). Among T1a drinkers, low-level use was most frequently endorsed (22.0%), followed by binge (15.9%) and heavy drinking (14.0%), respectively. There was a marked change in drinking patterns in T1b. Notably, abstinence increased from 38.8 to 71.2%, and of those who did report drinking alcohol, most did so at low-levels (16.9%). Binge and heavy drinking decreased to 3.1 and 1.8%, respectively. This trend remained consistent through T2 and T3, although some women did return to low-level drinking as their pregnancy progressed (low-level drinkers: T2, 25.6%; T3, 25.5%).

Patterns of alcohol use by partners

Additional file 1: Table S1 shows the pattern of alcohol use reported by partners. Among those who drank (85.6%), binge drinking was most common (27.9%), followed by low (20.3%), moderate (14.4%) and heavy drinking (13.3%), respectively.

Characteristics associated with maternal drinking in pregnancy

Univariate tests compared whether abstainers and pregnancy drinkers (at any level) differed on background socio-demographics, other substance use, and physical and psychological factors (Table 3). The results show that, relative to abstainers, women who drank alcohol had greater odds of being older (e.g., 30–35 years, 1.97, 95% CI, 1.20–3.24); completing high school (2.61, 95% CI, 1.48–4.61); having moderate (2.29, 95% CI, 1.31–4.02) or high SES (4.42, 95% CI, 2.56–7.64); being born in an English speaking country (1.88, 95% CI, 1.33–2.66); living in a household with multiple parents (single parent: 0.61, 95% CI, 0.39–0.95); and speaking English as their first language (2.34, 95% CI, 1.77–3.09); and lower odds of living in a single parent household (0.61, 95% CI, 0.39-0.95). Other factors associated with pregnancy drinking included: smoking in pregnancy (1.67, 95% CI, 1.18–2.36); reduced anxiety (0.76, 95% CI, 0.57–0.99); and higher estimated IQ (e.g., a score of 100–114, 3.02, 95% CI, 2.01–4.53).

Table 3 Maternal and Infant Factors Associated with Alcohol Use (Pooled Data, N = 1324)

Univariate tests also compared whether infants of abstainers and pregnancy drinkers (at any level) differed on sex and birth indicators (Table 3). Compared to infants of abstainers, infants born to mothers who drank in pregnancy were less likely to be born preterm (< 36 weeks gestation; 0.05, 95% CI, 0.3–0.82). No significant differences were found in sex, birthweight, head circumference or Apgar scores.

Characteristics associated with paternal drinking

Additional file 1: Table S2 shows the results of univariate tests comparing the characteristics of partners of abstainers with partners of pregnancy drinkers. Compared to partners of abstainers, partners of drinkers had three-fold greater odds of being low-level drinkers (2.99, 95% CI, 1.79–5.00); four-fold greater odds of being moderate drinkers (4.27, 95% CI, 2.40–7.63); six-fold greater odds of being binge drinkers (6.08, 95% CI, 3.65–10.12); and almost eight-fold greater odds of being heavy drinkers (7.78, 95% CI, 4.06–14.91). Partners of drinkers were older (e.g., 30–35 years, 2.89 95% CI, 1.52–5.52), less likely to report a non-English speaking background (0.49, 95% CI, 0.34–0.72); and more likely to report English as their first language (2.08, 95% CI, 1.40–3.11), compared to partners of abstainers.

PAE and infant GM development

Regression analyses were used to examine the relationship between PAE exposure and infant GM development at 12-months (Table 4).

Table 4 Regression Results For Maternal Alcohol Use and Infant Gross Motor Outcomes (Pooled Data, N = 1324)

For T1a, in the unadjusted analyses, there were no significant associations between PAE and GM outcomes. This relationship remained unchanged after adjustment. For T1b, T2 and T3, the results were consistent when PAE exposure was binary (i.e., abstinence versus low-level drinking); namely, low PAE was not significantly associated with GM development in infants at 12-months in the unadjusted, nor in the adjusted analyses. Table 5 shows the marginal means and 95% CIs for PAE and GM outcomes at all levels of adjustment.

Table 5 Marginal means for maternal alcohol use and infant gross motor outcomes (pooled data, N = 1324)

As paternal factors have been associated with maternal drinking in pregnancy, a second series of regression analyses were conducted within a sub-sample of the women whose partners participated in the study (Additional file 1: Tables S3 and S4). Again, PAE was not significantly associated with GM development at 12-months (See Additional file 1: Section A for sensitivity analyses).

Propensity score matching: low level exposure versus no exposure

Using propensity score matching, 308 abstinent mothers were matched to 312 low-level drinkers in Trimester 2. Results indicated that GM scores did not differ significantly between children born to drinkers (M = 9.20, SD = 2.85) and those born to abstainers (M = 8.82, SD = 2.52; t = − 1.75, p = 0.08).

The sample was then stratified into two levels of risk based on the propensity score matching (see Additional file 2: Section B). Highest risk of alcohol exposure was found to be related to factors including higher SES, higher education, older age, being of English speaking origin, tobacco use, and unplanned pregnancy. In the highest risk subgroup, 122 abstainers were matched to 195 drinkers. In the lowest risk subgroup, there were 186 abstinent women matched to 117 drinkers. Significant differences were only observed between drinkers and abstainers in the highest risk group (t = − 2.92, p = 0.004), with children born to low-level drinkers having higher GM scores (M = 9.20, SD = 2.85) compared to those born to abstainers (M = 8.82, SD = 2.52) (see Additional file 2: Section B Figure S1).

Discussion

This study used unique, multi-wave data on 1324 infant offspring from a longitudinal pregnancy cohort to examine the association between PAE and infant GM development at 12-months. The study specifically addressed PAE timing (i.e., four time-points in pregnancy) and dose (i.e., low, binge, moderate and heavy PAE in T1a; and low-level PAE thereafter). Results showed that alcohol use was common in pregnancy, particularly in the first 6-weeks, when parents were unaware of their pregnancy. Thereafter, any drinking by pregnant women generally occurred at low-levels (≤7 standard drinks per week, up to 2 standard drinks per occasion). No significant differences in BSID-III GM scale scores were identified among infants of abstainers compared with infants whose mothers reported any alcohol consumption in pregnancy, before or after adjustment for potential confounders.

Patterns of pregnancy drinking: mothers and partners

Pregnancy drinking was common in this cohort: 56.1% of pregnant women drank in Trimester one, 0–6 weeks (T1a); 23.6% in Trimester one, 7–12 weeks (T1b); 30.5% in Trimester two (T2); and, 29.6% in Trimester three (T3), respectively. Most women reported drinking at low-levels (average 22.5% across pregnancy). With the exception of T1a, few women reported moderate (2.3%), binge (1.7%) or heavy (1.3%) drinking in pregnancy. Whilst this is consistent with past research, greater specificity in this cohort on PAE timing and dose highlights two findings of public health import [36, 37]. First, rates of drinking prior to pregnancy awareness are close to twice that following awareness; and second, binge and heavy drinking both occur at high rates within the pre-awareness period: 15.9 and 14.0%, respectively. Taken together, the very earliest period of pregnancy may be one of greatest risk of exposure to alcohol.

Characteristics of women drinking in pregnancy and their partners

Consistent with past research, pregnant women who consumed alcohol differed on socio-demographic characteristics compared to abstainers [36, 37]. Specifically, they were more likely to be older, tertiary educated, have moderate to high SEIFA scores (reflective of socio-economic advantage), be born in Australia or another English speaking country, and less likely to live in a multiple parent household. Other factors associated with pregnancy drinking included: smoking in pregnancy; reduced anxiety; higher estimated IQ; and lower odds of obesity. These results suggest pregnancy drinking is common among women from more affluent socio-demographic backgrounds, and among specific at-risk groups such as women who smoke cigarettes. Targeting these populations may result in more effective preventive intervention for pregnancy drinking.

With respect to partners, our results are consistent with the limited extant literature [38, 39]. Specifically, partners of pregnant women who drink were more likely to be from advantaged socio-economic backgrounds (higher SEIFA scores and educational attainment), and to also drink alcohol and smoke tobacco. These characteristics may affect offspring development via their influence within the familial environment (i.e., partner drinking increases the risk for maternal drinking) [40]. Few studies have accounted for these potential influences when examining associations between PAE and offspring development [38].

PAE and infant GM development

The third aim was to determine whether GM development was impaired among infants exposed to PAE compared with infant offspring of abstainers. Potential background socio-demographic confounders were included in the adjusted analyses, along with other potential confounders (maternal substance use, physical and psychological factors; infant factors) associated with PAE exposure in the univariate analyses. Finally, to account for the potential role of partner factors in determining infant outcomes, we re-ran the models in a sub-set of the sample for whom partner data were available. We found no evidence to suggest that low PAE was associated with measurable impairments in infant GM development at 12-months. Moreover, at T1a (0–6 weeks), prior to pregnancy awareness for most women, neither moderate, binge nor heavy drinking predicted measurable GM impairment. In all models, and at all levels of adjustment, the pattern of results remained unchanged.

With regard to low-level exposure, the results are consistent with a small number of existing studies showing GM impairment was not linked to low PAE [20, 21]. There are a number of plausible explanations for this finding. First, that low PAE does not have a deleterious effect on early GM development. Alternatively, if harm does occur, the effects are likely to be very small. Our current measurement instruments, even though gold-standard, may not be sufficiently sensitive to detect such small GM deficits. Finally, it is well documented that infant development may fluctuate during infancy [41]. If low PAE does have harmful effects on GM development, these effects may be more readily detected in childhood as GM skills stabilise.

Importantly, when the sample was stratified by propensity score, the relationship between alcohol exposure and GM outcome was different relative to women’s baseline characteristics. In the highest risk subgroup, significant differences were observed between low-level drinkers and abstainers, with children born to low-level drinkers having higher GM scores compared to those born to abstainers. Consistent with other epidemiological findings [35], this result is suggestive of a potential interaction between PAE and other demographic and maternal risk factors in relation to offspring development, Further assessment of this interaction is recommended in samples with greater representation of women from low SES and high-risk backgrounds.

The finding that neither binge, moderate nor heavy PAE were linked to poorer GM development is inconsistent with a number of studies [2]. We did not examine potential harms after T1a due to the low frequency of these drinking patterns. It is possible that harmful drinking patterns later in pregnancy or persistent patterns of harmful drinking across the gestational window may be associated with GM impairment. It has been documented that there are sensitive gestational periods where risk for negative outcomes from teratogens may be heightened [42]. Differential impacts (relative to exposure timing) may explain inconsistency in the literature, and why it is difficult to determine a specific threshold at which PAE is harmful or safe for fetal development. Future work with greater representation of moderate, binge and heavy drinking patterns across the gestational period is needed, either through new cohorts with larger samples, targeted samples of moderate to higher-risk drinkers, and/or through potential data pooling/harmonisation [43].

Limitations

There are a number of limitations. First, women with low SES backgrounds (and their partners) were underrepresented, and those who were included tended to be abstainers rather than drinkers. As such, this study may not have captured low SES and marginalised families, whose children may be most susceptible to harms relating to PAE [44]. When the sample was stratified by propensity score, the relationship between alcohol exposure and GM outcome was different for women with different patterns of baseline characteristics. Results should thus be interpreted with the caveat that associations between alcohol use and GM outcomes may show a different pattern among lower SES or high-risk populations. Targeted recruitment of these disadvantaged families may result in better representation of the effects of heavier PAE patterns across a range of demographics. Obtaining sufficient representation of heavier PAE post-awareness is difficult due to the reduction that occurs in drinking. Data pooling and/or harmonisation of existing cohorts might be one approach to address this issue [43]. Nevertheless, this study did have good representation of varying PAE within the first six weeks of pregnancy, particularly low PAE from T1a to birth, suggesting that the cohort was well-suited to the assessment of impacts of low PAE. Second, despite the use of the gold-standard, clinically administered BSID-III, GM skills can fluctuate in infancy [41]. Potential PAE effects not identified in this study may emerge as the cohort offspring develop and their capacities stabilise. Thus, follow-up of the cohort to assess development into childhood is important.

Conclusion

This large-scale prospective study, with detailed assessment of PAE timing and dose, found no evidence to suggest that low PAE is associated with measurable impairment in infant GM development at 12-months. This result appears consistent with the limited available research [2]. Examination of higher exposure levels found that in T1a, prior to pregnancy awareness, neither moderate, binge, nor heavy PAE were associated with offspring GM impairment. Since most women either ceased or reduced their alcohol consumption following pregnancy awareness, this study was not able to examine the consequences of heavier drinking through pregnancy. We note that the present study focused on one developmental time-point; given the variability in GM development through infancy, it is possible that deleterious effects may be observed later in childhood. Further research is needed to examine potential PAE impacts on GM development through childhood.

Change history

  • 04 July 2019

    Following publication of the original article [1], the authors opted to revise the first paragraph of the section “Characteristics associated with maternal drinking in pregnancy”. Below is the updated version.

Abbreviations

BSID-III:

The Bayely Scales of Infant Development, Third edition

GM:

Gross motor

PAE:

Prenatal alcohol exposure

T1a:

Trimester one, 0–6 weeks

T1b:

Trimester one, 7–12 weeks

T2:

Trimester two, 13–27 weeks

T3:

Trimester three, 28+ weeks

References

  1. Bay B, Kesmodel US. Prenatal alcohol exposure - a systematic review of the effects on child motor function. Acta Obstet Gynecol Scand. 2011;90:210–26.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Lucas BR, Latimer J, Pinto RZ, Ferreira ML, Doney R, Lau M, et al. Gross motor deficits in children prenatally exposed to alcohol: a meta-analysis. Pediatrics. 2014;134:e192–209.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Jones KL, Smith D. Recognition of the fetal alcohol syndrome in early infancy. Lancet. 1973;302:999–1001.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  4. Jones KL, Streissguth AP. Fetal alcohol syndrome and fetal alcohol spectrum disorders: a brief history. J Psychiatry Law. 2010;38 Winter:373–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Marcus JC. Neurological findings in the fetal alcohol syndrome. Neuropediatrics. 1987;18:158–60.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  6. Smith DF, Sandor GG, MacLeod PM, Tredwell S, Wood B, Newman DE. Intrinsic defects in the fetal alcohol syndrome: studies on 76 cases from British Columbia and the Yukon territory. Neurobehav Toxicol Teratol. 1981;3:145–52.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Osborn JA, Harris SR, Weinberg J. Fetal alcohol syndrome: review of the literature with implications for physical therapists. Phys Ther. 1993;73:599–607.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  8. Jones KL, Hoyme HE, Robinson LK, Del Campo M, Manning MA, Prewitt LM, et al. Fetal alcohol spectrum disorders: extending the range of structural defects. Am J Med Genet Part A. 2010;152:2731–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Autti-Rämö I, Autti T, Korkman M, Kettunen S, Salonen O, Valanne L. MRI findings in children with school problems who had been exposed prenatally to alcohol. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2002;44:98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Riley EP, Mattson SN, Sowell ER, Jernigan TL, Sobel DF, Jones KL. Abnormalities of the corpus callosum in children prenatally exposed to alcohol. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 1995;19:1198–202.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  11. Mattson SN, Riley EP, Sowell ER, Jernigan TL, Sobel DF, Jones KL. A decrease in the size of the basal ganglia in children with fetal alcohol syndrome. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 1996;20:1088–93.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  12. Bookstein FL, Streissguth AP, Sampson PD, Connor PD, Barr HM. Corpus callosum shape and neuropsychological deficits in adult males with heavy fetal alcohol exposure. Neuroimage. 2002;15:233–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Korkman M, Kettunen S, Autti-Ramo I. Neurocognitive impairment in early adolescence following prenatal alcohol exposure of varying duration. Neuropsychol Dev Cogn C Child Neuropsychol. 2003;9:117–28.

    Google Scholar 

  14. Chaudhuri JD. Myelin degeneration in peripheral nerve in chick embryos following continuous ethanol exposure during early gestational period: a preliminary report. Neuroanatomy. 2006;5:50–5.

    Google Scholar 

  15. Ramadoss J, Lunde ER, Chen WJA, West JR, Cudd TA. Temporal vulnerability of fetal cerebellar purkinje cells to chronic binge alcohol exposure: ovine model. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2007;31:1738–45.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  16. Miller MW. Exposure to ethanol during gastrulation alters somatosensory-motor cortices and the underlying white matter in the macaque. Cereb Cortex. 2007;17:2961–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Schneider ML, Moore CF, Becker EF. Timing of moderate alcohol exposure during pregnancy and neonatal outcome in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2001;25:1238–45.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  18. Flak AL, Su S, Bertrand J, Denny CH, Kesmodel US, Cogswell ME. The association of mild, moderate, and binge prenatal alcohol exposure and child neuropsychological outcomes: a meta-analysis. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2014;38:214–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Gallahue D, Ozmun J, Goodway J. Understanding motor development: infants, children, adolescents, adults. 7th ed. Boston: McGraw-Hill; 2011.

    Google Scholar 

  20. Bay B, Støvring H, Wimberley T, Denny CH, Mortensen EL, Eriksen HLF, et al. Low to moderate alcohol intake during pregnancy and risk of psychomotor deficits. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2012;36:807–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Mattson SN, Riley EP. A review of the neurobehavioral deficits in children with fetal alcohol syndrome or prenatal exposure to alcohol. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 1998;22:279–94.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  22. Bayley N, Reuner G. Bayley scales of infant and toddler development: Bayley-III (Vol. 7). San Antonio, USA: Harcourt Assessment, Pearson Psych. Corporation; 2006.

  23. Hutchinson D, Wilson J, Allsop S, Elliott E, Najman J, Burns L, Bartu A, Jacobs S, Honan I, McCormack C, Rossen L. Cohort Profile: The Triple B Pregnancy Cohort Study: A longitudinal study of the relationship between alcohol, tobacco and other substance use during pregnancy and the health and well-being of Australian children and families. International journal of epidemiology. 2017;47(1):26–7m.

  24. McCormack C, Hutchinson D, Burns L, Wilson J, Elliott E, Allsop S, et al. Prenatal alcohol consumption between conception and recognition of pregnancy. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2017;41:369–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. O’Leary CM, Bower C, Zubrick SR, Geelhoed E, Kurinczuk JJ, Nassar N. A new method of prenatal alcohol classification accounting for dose, pattern and timing of exposure: improving our ability to examine fetal effects from low to moderate alcohol. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2010;64:956–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Cox JL, Holden JM, Sagovsky R. Detection of postnatal depression: development of the 10-item Edinburgh postnatal depression scale. Br J Psychiatry. 1987;150(June):782–6.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  27. Lovibond S, Lovibond P. Manual for the depression anxiety stress scales. Sydney, Australia: Psychology Foundation; 1995.

  28. Hudson WW, McIntosh SR. The assessment of spouse abuse: two quantifiable dimensions. J Marriage Fam. 1981;43:873.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Wechsler D. The test of premorbid functioning. San Antonio, TX. In: USA: the psychological corporation; 2009.

    Google Scholar 

  30. SPSS. IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows Version 22.0. 2013.

    Google Scholar 

  31. Stata. Stata Statistical Software Version 14.0. 2015.

    Google Scholar 

  32. Graham JW. Missing data analysis: making it work in the real world. Annu Rev Psychol. 2009;60:549–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Rubin D. Multiple imputation for nonresponse in surveys. New York: John Wiley & Sons; 2004.

    Google Scholar 

  34. Hosmer J, Lemeshow S, Sturdivant R. Model-building strategies and methods for logistic regression. In: Applied logistic regression. 3rd edition. Hoboken, NJ, USA: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.; 2013. p. 89–151.

  35. McCormack C, Hutchinson D, Burns L, Youssef G, Wilson J, Elliott E, et al. Maternal and partner prenatal alcohol use and infant cognitive development. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2018;185:330–8.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  36. Hutchinson D, Moore EA, Breen C, Burns L, Mattick RP. Alcohol use in pregnancy: prevalence and predictors in the longitudinal study of Australian children. Drug Alcohol Rev. 2013;32:475–82.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. O’Keeffe LM, Kearney PM, McCarthy FP, Khashan AS, Greene RA, North RA, et al. Prevalence and predictors of alcohol use during pregnancy: findings from international multicentre cohort studies. BMJ Open. 2015;5:e006323.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Alati R, Davey Smith G, Lewis SJ, Sayal K, Draper ES, Golding J, et al. Effect of prenatal alcohol exposure on childhood academic outcomes: contrasting maternal and paternal associations in the ALSPAC study. PLoS One. 2013;8:e74844.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  39. Skagerstróm J, Chang G, Nilsen P. Predictors of drinking during pregnancy: a systematic review. J Women's Health. 2011;20:901–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Hutchinson D, Mattick R, Braunstein D. The impact of alcohol use disorders on family life: a review of the empirical literature. Sydney; 2014.

  41. Darrah J, Redfern L, Maguire TO, Beaulne AP, Watt J. Intra-individual stability of rate of gross motor development in full-term infants. Early Hum Dev. 1998;52:169–79.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  42. Sawada Feldman H, Lyons Jones K, Lindsay S, Slymen D, Klonoff-Cohen H, Kao K, et al. Prenatal alcohol exposure patterns and alcohol-related birth defects and growth deficiencies: a prospective study. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2012;36:670–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Hutchinson D, Silins E, Mattick RP, Patton GC, Fergusson DM, Hayatbakhsh R, et al. How can data harmonisation benefit mental health research? An example of the Cannabis cohorts research consortium. Aust New Zeal J Psychiatry. 2015;49:317–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Esper LH, Furtado EF. Identifying maternal risk factors associated with fetal alcohol Spectrum disorders: a systematic review. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2014;23:877–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Australian Bureau of Statistics. 2033.0.55.001 - Census of population and housing: socio-economic indexes for areas (SEIFA), Australia, 2011. ICPSR Data Holdings. 2013.

  46. Holdnack JA, Drozdick L, Weiss LG, Iverson GL. WAIS-IV, WMS-IV, and ACS: advanced clinical interpretation. Academic Press; 2013.

  47. Creinin MD, Simhan HN. Can we communicate gravidity and parity better? Obstet Gynecol. 2009;113:709–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. World Health Organisation. Global database on body mass index. Secondary global database on body mass index 2006.

  49. World Health Organization. Obesity: preventing and managing the global epidemic. Geneva: WHO; 2000.

    Google Scholar 

  50. National Health and Medical Research Council. Australian guidelines to reduce health risks from drinking alcohol. Canberra: Commonwealth of Australia; 2009.

    Google Scholar 

  51. Dobbins TA, Sullivan EA, Roberts CL, Simpson JM. Australian national birthweight percentiles by sex and gestational age, 1998-2007. Med J Aust. 2012;197:291–4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We gratefully acknowledge the NDARC and NDRI research staff and students who assisted with collection of the data, study investigators not included as authors, the hospitals and antenatal clinics for their assistance with recruitment, and the study participants and their families. We also wish to acknowledge the Cannabis Cohorts Research Consortium (CCRC; NHMRC Project Grants: AAP1009381, AAP1064893).

Funding

The research was funded by an Australian National Health and Medical Research Council (NHMRC) Project Grant #GNT630517 for $2,196,179 to Richard P Mattick, Delyse Hutchinson, Steve Allsop, Jake Najman, Elizabeth Elliott, Lucinda Burns, Sue Jacobs, Craig Olsson and Anne Bartu, and was financially supported by the National Drug and Alcohol Research Centre (NDARC), University of New South Wales (UNSW). The cohort is led by the National Drug and Alcohol Research Centre (NDARC) at UNSW Australia, and the Drug Research Institute (NDRI) at Curtin University, in collaboration with Deakin University, Sydney University, the University of Queensland, the University of Christchurch, and the Murdoch Childrens Research Institute. NDARC and the National Drug Research Institute (NDRI), Curtin University of Technology are funded by the Australian Government under the Substance Misuse Prevention and Service Improvements Grants Fund. The study has also been supported by Australian Rotary Health (ARH; 2012–2013) the Foundation for Alcohol Research and Education (FARE; 2010–2011), and the Financial Markets Foundation for Children (Australia) (2015–2016). Additionally, PhD candidates on the project have been funded through ARH; the NDARC Education Trust (NET) and the Australian Centre for Perinatal Science and NDARC, UNSW. EE is supported by an NHMRC Practitioner Fellowship #1021480; CAO is financially supported by an Australian Research Council Principal Research Fellowship; and, RPM is financially supported by an NHMRC Principal Research Fellowship Award from the NHMRC.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

DH conceptualised and designed the study, coordinated and supervised data collection, carried out the analyses, contributed to the interpretation of the data, and wrote the manuscript. GY carried out the analyses, and contributed to the interpretation of the data, the write-up of the results and preparation of tables, and revision of the manuscript. CM contributed to the collection of data, data analysis, interpretation of the results, and the write-up and revision of the manuscript. JW supervised data collection, prepared the data for analysis, carried out the analyses, and contributed to the interpretation of the data and revision of the manuscript. SA and LB conceptualised and designed the study, coordinated and supervised data collection, and critically reviewed and revised the manuscript for important intellectual content. JN, EE and SJ conceptualised and designed the study and critically reviewed and revised the manuscript for important intellectual content. IH, LR, HF and ST contributed to the collection of data, and critically reviewed and revised the manuscript for important intellectual content. JR contributed to data management, and critically reviewed and revised the manuscript for important intellectual content. CO contributed to data analysis, interpretation of the results, and critically reviewed and revised the manuscript for important intellectual content. RM conceptualised and designed the study, coordinated and supervised data collection and the preparation of data for analysis, and contributed to the interpretation of the data and revision of the manuscript. All authors approved the final manuscript as submitted and agree to be accountable for all aspects of the work.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Delyse Hutchinson.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Ethics approval was granted by the Sydney South West Area Health Service Human Research Ethics Committee and the University of New South Wales Human Research Ethics Committee (Reference: HC08224; 29/08/2008). Written informed consent was obtained from all participants.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Additional files

Additional file 1:

Section A Table S1. Patterns of alcohol use by partners across pregnancy. Table S2. Paternal factors associated with alcohol use by mothers. Table S3. Regression results for maternal alcohol use and infant gross motor outcomes; women with partner in study only. Table S4. Marginal means for maternal alcohol use and infant gross motor outcomes; women with partner in study only. (DOCX 24 kb)

Additional file 2:

Section B Description of propensity score matching analysis. Figure S1. Infant gross motor score in low prenatal alcohol exposure vs. abstinent group, stratified by propensity score. (DOCX 20 kb)

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Hutchinson, D., Youssef, G.J., McCormack, C. et al. Prenatal alcohol exposure and infant gross motor development: a prospective cohort study. BMC Pediatr 19, 149 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12887-019-1516-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12887-019-1516-5

Keywords